You are viewing a javascript disabled version of the site. Please enable Javascript for this site to function properly.
Go to headerGo to navigationGo to searchGo to contentsGo to footer
In content section. Select this link to jump to navigation

Concentration Endurance Test (d2): Normative data for Spanish-speaking pediatric population

Abstract

OBJECTIVE:

To generate normative data for the Concentration Endurance Test (d2) in Spanish-speaking pediatric populations.

METHOD:

The sample consisted of 4,373 healthy children from nine countries in Latin America (Chile, Cuba, Ecuador, Guatemala, Honduras, Mexico, Paraguay, Peru, and Puerto Rico) and Spain. Each participant was administered the d2 test as part of a larger neuropsychological battery. The Total number of items processed (TN), Total number of correct responses (CR), Total performance (TP), and Concentration performance (CP) scores were normed using multiple linear regressions and standard deviations of residual values. Age, age2, sex, and mean level of parental education (MLPE) were included as predictors in the analyses.

RESULTS:

The final multiple linear regression models showed main effects for age on all scores, such that scores increased linearly as a function of age. TN scores were affected by age2 for Guatemala and Puerto Rico; CR scores were affected by age2 for Mexico; TP scores were affected by age2 for Chile, Mexico, Puerto Rico, and Spain; and CP scores for Mexico and Spain. Models indicated that children whose parents had a MLPE >12 years obtained higher scores compared to children whose parents had a MLPE≤12 years for Mexico and Spain in all scores, and Puerto Rico for TN, CR, and TP, and Guatemala and Paraguay for CP scores. Sex affect the scores for Ecuador and Honduras (CP scores).

CONCLUSIONS:

This is the largest Spanish-speaking pediatric normative study in the world, and it will allow neuropsychologists from these countries to have a more accurate approach to interpret the d2 test in pediatric populations.

1Introduction

The term ‘attention’ refers to a multifaceted and multidimensional cognitive construct that includes sustained attention, selective attention, and inhibitory control (Lehman, Naglieri, & Aguilino, 2010). Prior research has demonstrated that up to 40% of children aged four years old already exhibit difficulties with attention to warrant teacher and parent concern (Palfrey, Levine, Walker, & Sullivan, 1985). Moreover, Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder has emerged as one of the most common childhood conditions and one of the top reasons for neuropsychological referral (Sweet, Benson, Nelson, & Moberg, 2015). Given that attention plays a fundamental role in all cognitive, social, and emotional processes, impairments in these skills are associated with poor adaptive functioning, reduced quality of life, and a variety of academic problems (Isquith, Crawford, Espy, & Gioia, 2005; Miller & Hinshaw, 2010). Thus, there has been significant interest in the development of various instruments to measure attention in children such as parent rating questionnaires, continuous performance tests, and visual discrimination tests so that early and effective interventions can be provided.

The d2 test of attention is a paper-and-pencil measure of selective attention, concentration, and speeded visual perceptual discrimination that has been widely used in pediatric populations (Brickenkamp, 1994). The individual is asked to identify relevant targets (the letter d with two apostrophe marks that may be located above, both below, or one above and one below the d), while ignoring/suppressing irrelevant distracters, including orthographically similar stimuli (the letter d with one, three or four marks as well as the letter p with one or two marks). The stimuli are organized in 14 rows of 47 letters each (658 total elements, 229 relevant targets). Task instructions require the individual to scan each row from left to right for 20 seconds crossing out as many relevant elements as possible before the examiner prompts him or her to move to the subsequent row. Total test duration is between 8 and 10 minutes (Brickenkamp, 1981).

Percent rank scores based on age-based norms are calculated from the individual’s task performance. The following scales include: 1) speed which derives from the total number of correctly identified relevant responses, 2) accuracy which is based on the number of omission and commission errors, 3) total which measures the number of correct responses subtracting errors, and 4) variability or fluctuation rate which is based on the difference in correct responses between the rows with the highest and lowest number of correct responses (Brickenkamp, 1981).

The d2 test has also shown to have strong psychometric properties with high values of internal consistency, ranging between 0.95 and 0.98, and a validity coefficient of 0.47 (Bates & Lemay, 2004; Brickenkamp, Schmidt-Atzert, Liepmann, & Zillmer, 2013). The Spanish adaptation of the d2 test has similarly shown high reliability coefficient values of around 0.90 (Izquierdo et al., 2007). A convergent validity study with children aged 8–12 years old revealed high correlation between the overall d2 measure with other instruments for assessing attention in children such as the Faces Differences Perception Test (DPT) and Trees Simple Visual Discrimination Test (DiViSA-UAM). Notably, the omission errors index of the d2 test did not correlate significantly with any of the DPT or DIVISA measures (Lozano, Capote, & Fernandez, 2015). These findings may reflect the greater visual discrimination demands of the d2 in which individuals may be vulnerable to omission errors due to perceptual discrimination difficulties rather than attention lapses alone. Commission errors of the d2 showed significant correlations with the Faces-DPT (r = 0.236).

Demographic variables that may influence performance on the d2 test have been explored in pediatric populations, along with normative studies. Prior studies with the d2 and other measures of attention have not indicated significant differences according to sex, whether in attention or impulsivity (Klenberg, Korkman, & Lahti-Nuutila, 2001; Lin, Hsiao, & Chen, 1999; Quiroga, Santacreu, Montoro, Martínez-Molina, & Shih, 2011; Santacreu, Shih, & Quiroga, 2010). Normative data has been published for 1032 children from the Spanish Canary Islands who are 6–12 years old (Jiménez et al., 2012), showing significant developmental changes over time. A linear progression was observed, particularly in the areas of productivity and effectiveness but not in error measurement. In another study comparing the d2 to other attention measures such as the DiVisa-UAM and Faces Differences Perception Test, the d2 indicated a significant increase up to the 5th grade whereas the other two measures showed a progressive and significant increase in attention performance for children across school grades (Lozano et al., 2015). In contrast, the omission errors index of the d2 did not show a significant progression. Significant age differences were also not found for commission errors or impulsivity on the d2 test (Lozano et al., 2015). Culbertson and Sari (1997) conducted a pilot normative project of the d2 test with 54 American children, showing significantly greater productivity, accuracy, and variability performance with older age. Overall, results with the d2 test is consistent with evidence showing a significant progression in selective and sustained attention between eight and 12 years old, as well as stability of performance.

The d2 test has been used in various international pediatric clinical populations. Cserjési, Molnár, Luminet, and Lénárd (2007) found that Hungarian boys with obesity performed worse on the d2 test of attention when compared with their normal-weight same-aged peers, bolstering the evidence for childhood obesity leading to adverse effects on the development of attention. Recent evidence also suggests that children with low functioning autism spectrum disorders demonstrate reduced d2 performance in comparison to high functioning autism children, and that the degree of deficits was associated with abnormal neural connectivity (Han & Chan, 2017). A recent Brazilian study showed that children with prenatal alcohol exposure performed worse on the d2 test compared to same-aged peers in terms of speed, total errors, and omission errors (Furtado & Roriz, 2016). Begega and colleagues (2010) demonstrated no significant differences in d2 performance between preterm and full-term 8- and 9-year old children. The d2 has also been sensitive to attention difficulties often seen in epilepsy; clinical characteristics such as epilepsy duration and antiepileptic (AED) polytherapy have been associated with significantly worse d2 performance (Rahmann, Stodieck, Husstedt, & Evers, 2002).

More recently, the d2 test has also been utilized to measure the effectiveness of treatment programs in children. Fuchs, Birbaumer, Lutzenberger, Gruzelier, and Kaiser (2003) recently demonstrated that a 3-month neurofeedback program contingent on the suppression of theta/high beta and on the enhancement of SMR/low beta activity in EEG and pharmacotherapy was associated with improvements on the speed and accuracy measures of the d2. That is, children were able to work on more items while making fewer mistakes after treatment. Similarly, Mohammadi, Malmir, Khaleghi, and Aminiorani (2015) showed that neurofeedback along with methylphenidate led to improvements in d2 test performance. In the area of obesity and wellness, brief classroom-based exercise at low to moderate intensity had no acute effects on information processing speed and selective attention as measured by the d2 compared to a sedentary control condition in Dutch adolescents (van den Berg et al., 2016). However, Gallotta and colleagues (2015) demonstrated that obese and overweight children who participate in coordinative exercise at school exhibit significant improvements in attention/concentration and percentage of errors without improvements in total number of items processed on the d2 test. Even acute coordinative exercise has led to improvements in attention as measured by the d2 test (Budde, Voelcker-Rehage, Pietraßyk-Kendziorra, Ribeiro, & Tidow, 2008).

Previous normative studies of the d2 have been small or geographically limited; therefore, the lack of normative data could translate to erroneous clinical interpretation when evaluating Spanish-speaking or Hispanic/Latino children. Although there has been a Spanish adaptation of the d2 and it showed high internal consistency, norms for the d2 have not been established for children living in Latin America. Moreover, there is a dearth of adapted and well-normed tests available for use with Spanish speakers. In light of its brief duration and adequate psychometric properties, the main purpose of this study is to provide normative data for healthy children across nine Latin American countries and Spain in order to support its clinical utility.

2Method

2.1Participants

The sample consisted of 4,373 healthy children who were recruited from Chile, Cuba, Ecuador, Guatemala, Honduras, Mexico, Paraguay, Peru, Puerto Rico, and Spain. Participants were selected based on the following criteria: a) age between 6 and 17 years old, b) were born and currently lived in a country where the study was conducted, c) spoke Spanish as their mother tongue, d) an IQ ≥80 on the Test Of Non-verbal Intelligence (TONI-2, Brown, Sherbenou, & Johnsen, 2009), and e) a score <19 on the Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI, Kovacs, 1992).

Children with history of neurologic or psychiatric disorders, as reported by the participant’s parent(s), were excluded due to their effects on cognitive performance. Participants in the study were from public or private schools, and they signed an informed consent to participate. Socio-demographic and participant characteristics for each of the countries’ samples have been reported elsewhere (Rivera & Arango-Lasprilla, 2017). Ethics Committee approval was obtained for the study in each country.

2.2Instrument administration

d2 test is a measure of selective attention and mental concentration, as well as impulsivity (Wassenberg et al., 2008). It consists of 14 rows of 47 characters each, which can be the letters “d” or “p” with one, two, three, or four apostrophe marks located at the top and/or bottom of each letter. The examinee is asked to cross out the greater amount of “d” that have two apostrophe marks both above, both underneath, or one above and another below with 20 seconds allotted per row (Brickenkamp, 2012). The scores that are obtained in this test are Total number of items processed (TN), Total number of correct responses (CR), Omission errors (OE), Commission errors (CE), Total performance (TP), Concentration performance (CP) and Fluctuation rate (FR), and whose combination reflect three components of attentional behavior: the speed or quantity of work, the quality of the work (degree of precision) and the relationship between both (Wassenberg et al., 2008). In the present study, normative data for TN, CR, TP and CP will be provided.

2.3Statistical analyses

Detailed statistical analyses used to generate the normative data for the d2 test scores are described in Rivera & Arango-Lasprilla (2017). The scores were standardized using multiple linear regression analyses by means of a four-step procedure. 1) First, the TN, CR, TP, and CP scores were computed separately by means of the final multiple regression models. The full regression models included as predictors: age, age2, sex, and mean level of parental education (MLPE). Age was centered (= calendar age – mean age in the sample by country) before computing the quadratic age term to avoid multicollinearity (Aiken & West, 1991). Sex was coded as male = 1 and female = 0. The MLPE variable was coded as 1 if the participant’s parent(s) had >12 years of education or 0 if participant’s parent(s) had≤12 years of education. If predicted variables were not statistically significant in the multivariate model with an alpha of 0.05, the non-significant variables were removed and the model was run again. A final regression model was conducted yˆi=B0+B1·(Age-x¯Agebycountry)i+B2·(Age-x¯Agebycountry)i2+B3·Sexi+B4·MLPEi . 2) Residual scores were calculated based on the final model ( ei=yi-yˆi ). 3) Residuals were standardized using the residual Standard Deviation (SDe) value provided by the regression model: zi = ei/SDe. 4) Standardized residuals were converted to percentile values using the standard normal cumulative distribution function. This four-step process was applied to TN, CR, TP, and CP scores separately for each country.

For all multiple linear regression models, the following assumptions were evaluated: a) multicollinearity by the values of the Variance Inflation Factor (VIF), which must not exceed 10, and the collinearity tolerance values, which must not exceed the value of 1 (Kutner, Nachtsheim, Neter, & Li, 2005), and b) the existence of influential values by calculating the Cook’s distance. The maximum Cook’s distance value was related to a F (p, n - p) distribution. Influential values are considered when percentile value is equal or higher than 50 (Cook, 1977; Kutner et al., 2005). All analyses were performed using SPSS version 23 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY).

3Results

3.1d2 - Total number of items processed (TN)

The final multivariate linear regression models for the ten country-specific d2 TN scores were significant (see Table 1). In all countries, the d2 TN scores increased linearly as a function of age. The d2 TN score for Guatemala and Puerto Rico were affected by a quadratic age effect. Children from Mexico, Puerto Rico, and Spain whose parents had a MLPE >12 years obtained higher d2 TN scores than children whose parents had a MLPE ≤12 years. The child’s sex did not affect the d2 TN scores for any country. The amount of variance these predictors explained in the d2 TN scores ranged from 35.7% (in Puerto Rico) to 61.2% (in Mexico).

Table 1

Final multiple linear regression models for d2 TN

BStd. ErrortSig.R2SDe
(residual)
Chile
  Constant360.3913.97590.664<0.0010.50277.691
  Age22.4731.14819.584<0.001
Cuba
  Constant393.8804.65984.551<0.0010.57490.691
  Age30.4901.35222.550<0.001
Ecuador
  Constant369.5254.60980.179<0.0010.41879.291
  Age19.4511.33614.558<0.001
Guatemala
  Constant344.4766.20855.487<0.0010.43468.181
  Age22.0162.03310.830<0.001
  Age21.1750.5881.9980.047
Honduras
  Constant311.8533.71084.048<0.0010.58562.962
  Age23.1711.15220.115<0.001
Mexico
  Constant333.7203.52294.758<0.0010.61268.020
  Age24.4650.69835.075<0.001
  MLPE10.0304.8732.0580.040
Paraguay
  Constant374.8994.22488.760<0.0010.50173.029
  Age20.9411.21117.297<0.001
Peru
  Constant399.6275.27375.791<0.0010.53884.686
  Age26.9321.54117.472<0.001
Puerto Rico
  Constant367.59414.51425.327<0.0010.35788.271
  Age15.3751.9707.804<0.001
  Age2–1.6180.628–2.5760.011
  MLPE38.60514.5012.6620.008
Spain
  Constant337.5673.70891.029<0.0010.60867.847
  Age25.2180.66537.919<0.001
  MLPE13.7844.6462.9670.003

Note. MLPE: Mean level of parental education.

3.2d2 - Total number of correct responses (CR)

The final multivariate linear regression models for the ten country-specific d2 CR scores were significant (see Table 2). In all countries, the d2 CR scores increased linearly as a function of age. The d2 CR scores for Mexico were affected by a quadratic age effect. Children from Mexico, Puerto Rico, and Spain whose parents had a MLPE >12 years obtained higher d2 CR score than children whose parents had a MLPE ≤12 years. The child’s sex did not affect the d2 CR scores for any country. The amount of variance these predictors explained in the d2 CR scores ranged from 32.3% (in Puerto Rico) to 58.5% (in Cuba).

Table 2

Final multiple linear regression models for d2 CR

BStd. ErrortSig.R2SDe
(residual)
Chile
  Constant134.5951.51488.871<0.0010.52429.600
  Age8.9500.43720.470<0.001
Cuba
  Constant142.1652.10067.689<0.0010.58540.888
  Age14.0600.61023.066<0.001
Ecuador
  Constant131.7401.92368.505<0.0010.43633.085
  Age8.4150.55715.094<0.001
Guatemala
  Constant130.4232.06863.070<0.0010.35628.653
  Age8.3870.81610.278<0.001
Honduras
  Constant111.4591.56771.149<0.0010.51026.583
  Age8.4090.48617.291<0.001
Mexico
  Constant121.3022.07258.549<0.0010.54130.281
  Age9.3080.31129.955<0.001
  Age2–0.2440.102–2.3840.017
  MLPE10.2862.1804.719<0.001
Paraguay
  Constant133.2251.82473.037<0.0010.46031.538
  Age8.3270.52315.925<0.001
Peru
  Constant143.5172.22964.398<0.0010.54635.794
  Age11.5720.65217.762<0.001
Puerto Rico
  Constant130.3915.23324.916<0.0010.32337.521
  Age7.1410.7649.347<0.001
  MLPE13.1206.1452.1350.034
Spain
  Constant130.8031.62880.354<0.0010.56629.783
  Age10.1240.29234.680<0.001
  MLPE8.0692.0403.956<0.001

Note. MLPE: Mean level of parental education.

3.3d2 - Total performance (TP)

The final multivariate linear regression models for the ten country-specific d2 TP scores were significant (see Table 3). In all countries, the d2 TP scores increased linearly as a function of age. The d2 TP scores for Chile, Mexico, Puerto Rico, and Spain was affected by a quadratic age effect. Children from Mexico, Puerto Rico, and Spain whose parents had a MLPE >12 years obtained higher d2 TP scores than children whose parents had a MLPE ≤12 years. The child’s sex did not affect the d2 TP scores for any country. The amount of variance these predictors explained in the d2 TP scores ranged from 39.4% (in Puerto Rico) to 62.8% (in Mexico).

Table 3

Final multiple linear regression models for d2 TP

BStd. ErrortSig.R2SDe
(residual)
Chile
  Constant343.2905.21665.811<0.0010.58867.301
  Age23.1100.99523.217<0.001
  Age2–0.6560.326–2.0120.045
Cuba
  Constant357.5034.39481.367<0.0010.62585.535
  Age31.9981.27525.092<0.001
Ecuador
  Constant332.6144.04682.211<0.0010.51169.606
  Age20.5871.17317.553<0.001
Guatemala
  Constant324.0254.87066.540<0.0010.43267.473
  Age23.1431.92212.043<0.001
Honduras
  Constant284.3813.35184.857<0.0010.62056.867
  Age22.5101.04021.635<0.001
Mexico
  Constant307.0304.47668.588<0.0010.62865.426
  Age24.1860.67136.025<0.001
  Age2–0.5350.221–2.4150.016
  MLPE20.0344.7104.254<0.001
Paraguay
  Constant344.5725.92458.167<0.0010.55665.962
  Age21.0221.09619.188<0.001
Peru
  Constant364.1704.74276.798<0.0010.59576.160
  Age27.1921.38619.615<0.001
Puerto Rico
  Constant342.75013.73824.949<0.0010.39483.552
  Age16.1681.8658.669<0.001
  Age2–1.4940.594–2.5140.013
  MLPE34.40013.7262.5060.013
Spain
  Constant326.0624.42973.626<0.0010.60967.686
  Age25.2310.66637.899<0.001
  Age2–0.4310.214–2.0180.044
  MLPE17.2024.6383.709<0.001

Note. MLPE: Mean level of parental education.

3.4d2 - Concentration performance (CP)

The final multivariate linear regression models for the ten country-specific d2 CP scores were significant (see Table 4). In all countries, the d2 CP score increased linearly as a function of age. The d2 CP scores for Mexico and Spain were affected by a quadratic age effect. Children from Guatemala, Mexico, Paraguay, and Spain whose parents had a MLPE >12 years obtained higher d2 CP scores than children whose parents had a MLPE≤12 years. The child’s sex affected d2 CP scores for Ecuador and Honduras, favoring boys. The amount of variance these predictors explained in the d2 CP score ranged from 28.5% (in Puerto Rico) to 58.7% (in Cuba).

Table 4

Final multiple linear regression models for d2 CP

BStd. ErrortSig.R2SDe
(residual)
Chile
  Constant128.7351.60880.064<0.0010.55531.426
  Age10.1120.46421.785<0.001
Cuba
  Constant132.6112.26558.554<0.0010.58744.090
  Age15.2430.65723.189<0.001
Ecuador
  Constant118.2392.68244.092<0.0010.47135.035
  Age9.5190.59615.966<0.001
  Sex10.0814.1862.4080.017
Guatemala
  Constant118.5062.67044.388<0.0010.39031.794
  Age9.5780.91810.437<0.001
  MLPE10.5845.3431.9810.049
Honduras
  Constant101.9102.30044.301<0.0010.51628.421
  Age9.1040.52317.400<0.001
  Sex7.7033.3792.2800.023
Mexico
  Constant111.3822.34847.436<0.0010.51834.318
  Age9.9810.35228.344<0.001
  Age2–0.2880.116–2.4790.013
  MLPE14.6012.4705.911<0.001
Paraguay
  Constant114.6473.10136.970<0.0010.49331.515
  Age8.7910.52916.625<0.001
  MLPE8.3573.8582.1660.032
Peru
  Constant134.7612.42555.577<0.0010.51538.944
  Age11.8240.70916.681<0.001
Puerto Rico
  Constant131.5412.91845.075<0.0010.28541.669
  Age7.4030.8229.004<0.001
Spain
  Constant129.5012.05563.026<0.0010.55731.404
  Age10.5120.30934.031<0.001
  Age2–0.2450.099–2.4710.014
  MLPE9.4012.1524.369<0.001

Note. MLPE: Mean level of parental education.

The assumptions of multiple linear regression analysis were met for all final models. There was not multicollinearity (the VIF values were below 10; VIF ≤1.199; collinearity tolerance values did not exceed the value of 1) or influential cases (the maximum Cook’s distance value was 0.098 in a F (2,298)  distribution which corresponds to the 9th percentile).

3.5Normative procedure

Norms (e.g., a percentile score) for the different d2 test scores by country were established using the four-step procedure described in the statistical analysis section. An example will be provided to facilitate an improved understanding of the procedure used to obtain the percentile associated with a score on this test. Let us assume that we need to find the percentile score for a 17-year-old Puerto Rican girl who scored a 500 on the d2 TP and whose parent(s) have a mean of 6 years of education (MLPE). There are several steps to obtain the percentile for this score: First, find Puerto Rico in Table 3, which provides the final regression models by country for the d2 TP scores. Use the B weights to create an equation that will allow you to obtain the predicted d2 TP score for this child using the coding provided in the statistical analysis section. The corresponding B weights are multiplied by the centered age (= calendar age – mean age in the Puerto Rican sample which is equal to 12.2 years), centered age2 (= calendar age – mean age in the Puerto Rican sample which is equal to 12.2 years)2, and MLPE was coded based on the 12 years of education threshold. Gender was not a significant predictor, therefore, it is not included in the model. See Rivera and Arango-Lasprilla (2017) to figure out the mean age of each country’s sample. The result is subsequently added to the constant generated by the model in order to calculate the predicted value.

In the case of the Puerto Rican girl, the predicted d2 TP score would be calculated using the following equation: yˆi=342.750+[16.168·(Agei-12.2)]+[-1.494·(Agei-12.2)i2]+(34.400·MLPEi) . The girl’s age is 17 years old. The MLPE (6 years) is split into either ≤12 years (and assigned a 0) or more than 12 years (and assigned a 1) in the model. Since the parent(s) of the hypothetical child in the example have 6 years of education, the MLPE value is 0. As previously mentioned, sex was not significant predictor of d2 TP score, therefore, it should not be used in the formula. The predicted value equation is: yˆi=342.750+[16.168·(17-12.2)]+[-1.494·(17-12.2)2]+(34.400·0)=385.934 . In order to calculate the residual value (indicated with an ei in the equation), subtract the actual d2 TP score (she scored 500) from the predicted value just calculated ( ei=yi-yˆi ). In this case, it would be ei = 500 - 385.934 = 114.066. Next, consult the SDe column in Table 3 to obtain the country-specific SDe (residual) value. For Puerto Rico, it is 83.552. Using this value, we can transform the residual value to a standardized z score using the equation zi = ei/SDe. In this case, we have 114.066/83.552 = 1.365. This is the standardized z score for a 17-year-old Puerto Rican girl who scored a 500 on the d2 TP who has parents with 6 years of education (MLPE). The last step is to use the tables available in most statistical reference books (e.g., Strauss, Sherman, & Spreen, 2006). In this example, the z score of 1.365 corresponds to the 91st percentile. It is important to remember to use the appropriate tables that correspond to each test (TN, CR, TP, and CP) when performing these calculations.

3.6User-friendly normative data

The four-step normative procedures explained above offers the clinician the ability to determine an exact percentile for a child who has a specific score on the d2 test. However, this method can be prone to human error due to the number of required computations by hand. To enhance user-friendliness, the authors have completed these steps for a range of raw scores based on age, sex, and MLPE and created tables for clinicians to more easily obtain a percentile range/estimate associated with a given raw score on this test. These tables are available by country and type of test in the Appendix. In order to obtain an approximate percentile for the above example (converting a raw score of 500 on the d2 TP test for a Puerto Rican girl who is 17 years old and whose parent(s) have 6 years of education) using the simplified normative tables provided in the Appendix, the following steps must be followed: (1) First, identify the appropriate table ensuring the appropriate country and test (TN, CR, TP, and CP). In this case, the table for d2 TP score for Puerto Rico can be found in Table A29. (2) Next, the table is divided based on MLPE (≤12 vs. more than 12 years of education). Since the parent(s) had 6 years of education, we will use the bottom section of the table for ≤12 years of MLPE. (3) Find the appropriate age of the child, in this case, 17 years old. (4) Next, look in the 17 years’ age column to find the approximate location of the raw score obtained on the test. Within the 17 years’ column, the score of 500 obtained by this Puerto Rican girl corresponds to the 90th percentile.

The percentile obtained using this user-friendly table is slightly different than the hand-calculated, more accurate method (91st vs. 90th) because the user-friendly table is based on a limited number of percentile values. Individual percentiles cannot be presented in these tables due to space limitations. If the exact score is not listed in the column, you must estimate the percentile value from the list of raw scores available.

4Discussion

The purpose of this study was to obtain normative data for the d2 test for children and adolescents from nine Latin American countries (Chile, Cuba, Ecuador, Guatemala, Honduras, Mexico, Paraguay, Peru, and Puerto Rico) and Spain. The final regression models for the d2 test explained between 35.7% and 61.2% of the variance for the TN; between 32.3% and 58.5% of the variance for the CR; between 39.4% and 62.8% of the variance for the TP; and between 28.5% and 58.7% of the variance for the CP.

Age was significantly related to all d2 scores in all countries. That is, scores increased linearly as children became older. Similarly, Jiménez et al. (2012) found a linear effect of age in the measures of productivity and effectiveness obtained from the TN, CR, TP, and CP scores, and not in the error measures for a pediatric Spanish population. Culbertson and Sari (1997) obtained similar findings, showing that variability (FR) was also influenced by age. Studies have consistently shown an increase in attention capacity as children become older with neuropsychological tests other than the d2 (Korkman, Kemp, & Kirk, 2001; Rosselli, Ardila, Bateman, & Guzmán, 2001; Rosselli et al., 2004; Matute, Sanz, Gum, Rosselli, & Ardila, 2009). The explanation for these results can be found in the progressive development of the mechanisms involved in the inhibition of irrelevant information (Dempster, 1992; Gómez-Pérez, Ostrosky-Solís, & Próspero-García, 2003) as well as cortical maturation.

In the case of the quadratic function of age, the effect found was much lower than the linear function of age. That is, the results were only significant for the d2 TN in Guatemala and Puerto Rico. Results for the d2 CR were only significant for Mexico, and results for the d2 TP were significant for Chile, Mexico, Puerto Rico, and Spain. Finally, results for the d2 CP were only significant for Mexico and Spain. These results confirm that attentional processes usually develop in a linear fashion as children grow, or at least until the age of 17 years.

In the present study, sex was only significantly related to the CP scores in Ecuador and Honduras. However, in previous studies (Klengberg et al., 2001; Lin et al., 1999; Quiroga et al., 2011; Santacreu et al., 2010) sex was not related to d2 test scores. This shows that unlike other psychological processes such as language (Fenson et al., 1994; Huttenlocher, Haight, Bryk, Seltzer, & Lyons, 1991; Morisset, Barnard, & Booth, 1995) whose development occurs earlier in girls than in boys, attention seems to develop in a more stable manner over time in healthy children, regardless of sex.

Parental education has been shown to be a critical variable when investigating cognitive performance in children (Meador et al., 2011; Schady, 2011), especially language functions (Hoff & Tian, 2005; Hoff, Laursen, & Tardif, 2002). However, little consideration has been paid to the relationship between parental education and attention. Indeed, our study indicates that parental education significantly contributes to attentional skills. More specifically, the parent’s educational level was found to be significant for the four d2 scores in Mexico, Spain, and Puerto Rico (except for d2 CP), whereas in Guatemala and Paraguay for the CP, favoring children with higher parental education. Similar to our findings, Matute et al. (2009) demonstrated that lower parental education level was associated with reduced attention and memory performance in children, particularly in older children.

4.1Limitations and future directions

This study is the largest in the world that has been developed for the validation and standardization of the d2 test of attention in Spanish-speaking children. Nevertheless, it is important to mention some limitations related to the generalization and interpretation of our findings: This study presents normative data for the d2 test for nine countries from Latin America and Spain. For this reason, it is not advisable to use these norms in the pediatric population of those Spanish-speaking countries where the study was not performed. Future studies should be conducted to standardize this test in other Spanish-speaking countries.

Although the norms of the present study could be used by neuropsychologists in other countries to evaluate Spanish-speaking immigrant children from the countries where the sample was collected for this study, they should be used with caution since other variables such as level of acculturation, bilingualism, the number of years living in the country, and so on, could influence performance. In addition, the quality of education of both the child and the parents is another aspect that may influence the cognitive performance of children.

Although the d2 test is one of the most used instruments to evaluate attention, it is very important to keep in mind that no clinical diagnosis should be made based solely on the scores of this test alone. In combination with a clinical history, behavioral observations and objective questionnaires that measure attention skills in multiple settings, this test should be integrated as part of a comprehensive battery that evaluates these processes in detail. Because there are a limited number of tests and norms in Latin America and Spain to evaluate these processes, more efforts should be made in the future to develop other similar tools.

The size of the sample was adequate in each of the countries where the study was conducted. However, only the sample in Chile, Mexico, Paraguay, Puerto Rico, and Spain was obtained from several regions of the country whereas the samples from the remaining countries represented only one geographic area. Future studies should expand the sample in other geographical areas of these countries to increase the representativeness of the sample.

Finally, it is important to keep in mind that the present study was performed with a healthy and typically developing pediatric population. Therefore, future studies should be performed with clinical populations to establish the sensitivity and specificity of the d2 test in Spanish-speaking children.

4.2Implications and conclusions

This is the first and most extensive multicenter study carried out so far with the d2 test, offering normative data for children from nine Latin America countries and Spain. The current approach used for the creation and development of normative data in this study (the use of multiple regressions and residual values) is more precise than the use of average scores and standard deviations.

The addition of parental education and the quadratic function of age in the final regression models are some of the main advantages this study has in comparison to previous normative studies of the d2 test that have not taken these variables into account (e.g. Culbertson & Sari, 1997; Jiménez et al., 2012). Notably, the socio-demographic variables included in the model explain a high proportion of the variance of the d2 test scores (between 28.5% and 62.8%). In light of the high prevalence of attention problems in children and ADHD being the most common reason for referral for pediatric neuropsychological evaluation in Latin America, it is necessary to have adequate assessment tools to detect these problems and advance clinical decision making for diagnostic purposes. Therefore, the results of this study are expected to contribute to clinical practice across Latin America and Spain by improving the quality of how attention is assessed during routine pediatric neuropsychological evaluations.

Conflict of interest

None to report.

Supplementary material

[1] The Appendix tables are available in the electronic version of this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/NRE-172248.

References

1 

Aiken L. S. , & West S. G. ((1991) ), Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

2 

Bates M. E. , Lemay E. P. Jr. ((2004) ). The d2 test of attention: Construct validity and extensions in scoring techniques. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Neuropsychological Society, 10: , 392–400.

3 

Begega A. , Méndez López, M. , de Iscar M. J. , Cuesta-Izquierdo M. , Solís G. , Fernández-Colomer B. , & Arias J. L. ((2010) ). Assessment of the global intelligence and selective cognitive capacities in preterm 8-year-old children. Psicothema, 22: (4), 648–653.

4 

Brickenkamp R. ((1981) ), Test d2 Aufmerksamkeits-Belastungs-Test 7th ed. Gottingen: Verlag ü Psychologie (Hogrefe).

5 

Brickenkamp R. ((1994) ), Test d2 Aufmerksamkeits-Belastungs-Test. Handanweisung. 8th expanded and revised edition. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.

6 

Brickenkamp R. ((2012) ). Test de atención D2: Manual. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.

7 

Brickenkamp R. , Schmidt-Atzert L. , Liepmann D. , & Zillmer E. ((2013) ), d2- R Test di Attenzione Concentrata [d2-R Test of Sustained Attention]. Firenze: Hogrefe.

8 

Brown L. , & Sherbenou R. J. Johnsen, S. K. ((2009) ), Test de inteligencia no verbal TONI-2. Madrid: TEA ediciones.

9 

Budde H. , Voelcker-Rehage C. , Pietraßyk-Kendziorra S. , Ribeiro P. , & Tidow G. ((2008) ). Acute coordinative exercise improves attentional performance in adolescents. Neurosci Lett, 441: , 219–223. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008

10 

Cook R. D. ((1977) ). Detection of influential observation in linear regression. Technometrics, 19: (1), 15–18. doi: 10.2307/1268249

11 

Cserjési R. , Molnár D. , Luminet O. , & Lénárd L. ((2007) ). Isthereany relationship between obesity and mental flexibility in children? Appetite, 49: , 675–678. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2007.04.001

12 

Culbertson W. C. , & Sari D. ((1997) ). A preliminary normative study of the d2 Test with American children. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 4: , 303–304.

13 

Dempster F. N. ((1992) ). The rise and fall of the inhibitory mechanism: Toward a unified theory of cognitive development and aging. Developmental Review, 12: (1), 45–75.

14 

Fenson L. , Dale P. S. , Reznick J. S. , Bates E. , Thal D. J. , Pethick S. J. , & ... Stiles, J. ((1994) ). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the society for research in child development, i–185.

15 

Fuchs T. , Birbaumer N. , Lutzenberger W. , Gruzelier J. H. , & Kaiser J. ((2003) ). Neurofeedback treatment for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children: A comparison with methylphenidate. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 28: (1), 1–12. doi: 10.1023/A:1022353731579

16 

Furtado E. F. , & Roriz S. T. de S. ((2016) ). Inattention and impulsivity associated with prenatal alcohol exposure in a prospective cohort study with 11-years-old Brazilian children. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 25: (12), 1327–1335. doi: 10.1007/s00787-016-0857-y

17 

Gallotta M. C. , Emerenziani G. P. , Iazzoni S. , Meucci M. , Baldari C. , & Guidetti L. ((2015) ). Impacts of coordinative training on normal weight and overweight/obese children’s attentional performance. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9: , 577. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00577

18 

Gómez-Pérez E. , Ostrosky-Solís F. , & Próspero-García O. ((2003) ). Desarrollo de la atención, la memoria y los procesos inhibitorios: Relación temporal con la maduración de la estructura y función cerebral.a. Revista De Neurolog&rsquo;{ı, 37: (6), 561–567.

19 

Han Y. M. Y. , & Chan A. S. ((2017) ). Disordered cortical connectivity underlies the executive function deficits in children with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 61: , 19–31. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2016.12.010

20 

Hoff E. , & Tian C. ((2005) ). Socioeconomic status and cultural influences on language. Journal of communication Disorders, 38: (4), 271–278.

21 

Hoff E. , Laursen B. & Tardif T. ((2002) ). Socioeconomic status and parenting. In Bornstein M.H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting 231–252. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

22 

Huttenlocher J. , Haight W. , Bryk A. , Seltzer M. , & Lyons T. ((1991) ). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27: (2), 236.

23 

Isquith P. K. , Crawford J. S. , Espy K. A. , & Gioia G. A. ((2005) ). Assessment of executive function in preschool-aged children. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 11: (3), 209–215. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20075

24 

Izquierdo M. C. , Pérez M. J. D. I. , Losa M. A. B. , López M. M. , Alvarez Perez L. , Solís G. , & Pérez J. L. A. ((2007) ). Psychometric properties of the d2 selective attention test in a sample of premature and born-at-term babies. Psicothema, 19: (4), 706–710.

25 

Jiménez J. E. , Hernández S. , García E. , Díaz A. , Rodríguez C. , & Martín R. ((2012) ). Test de atención d2: Datos normativos y desarrollo evolutivo de la atención en educación primaria [Attention test D2: Normative da- ta and attention development in elementary grades]. European Journal of Educational Psychology, 5: , 93–106.

26 

Klenberg L. , Korkman M. , & Lahti-Nuutila P. ((2001) ). Differencial development of attention and executive functions in 3- to 12-year-old Finnish children. Developmental {Neuropsychology, 20: , 407–428.

27 

Korkman M. , Kemp S. L. , & Kirk U. ((2001) ). Effects of age on neurocognitive measures of children ages 5 to 12: A cross-sectional study on 800 children from the United States. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20: (1), 331–354.

28 

Kovacs M. ((1992) ). Children Depression Inventory CDI (Manual). Toronto: Multihealth systems

29 

Kutner M. H. , Nachtsheim C. J. , Neter J. , & Li W. ((2005) ), Applied linear statistical models (5th ed.). New York: McGraw Hill.

30 

Lehman E. B. , Naglieri J. A. , & Aquilino S. A. ((2010) ). A national study on the development of visual attention using the cognitive assessment system. J Atten Disord, 14: (1), 15–24. doi: 10.1177/1087054709332473 [pii].

31 

Lin C. C. H. , Hsiao C. K. , & Chen W. J. ((1999) ). Development of sustained attention assessed using the Continuous Performance Test among children 6-15 years of age. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 27: , 403–412.

32 

Lozano J. H. , Capote E. , & Fernández M. P. ((2015) ). Convergent validity of the measures of attention and impulsivity in the Trees: Simple Visual Discrimination Test (DiViSA-UAM), Anales de Psicologίa/Annals of Psychology, 31: (1), 74–83. doi: 10.6018/analesps.31.1.161371

33 

Meador K. J. , Baker G. A. , Browning N. , Clayton-Smith J. , Cohen M. J. , Kalayjian L. A. , & Loring D. W. ((2011) ). Relationship of child IQ to parental IQ and education in children with fetal antiepileptic drug exposure. Epilepsy & Behavior, 21: (2), 147–152.

34 

Miller M. , & Hinshaw S. P. ((2010) ). Does Childhood Executive Function Predict Adolescent Functional Outcomes in Girls with ADHD? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38: (3), 315–326. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9369-2

35 

Mohammadi M. R. , Malmir N. , Khaleghi A. , & Aminiorani M. ((2015) ). Comparison of Sensorimotor Rhythm (SMR) and Beta Training on Selective Attention and Symptoms in Children with Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): A Trend Report. Iranian Journal of Psychiatry, 10: (3), 147–152. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26877750.

36 

Morisset C. E. , Barnard K. E. , & Booth C. L. ((1995) ). Toddlers’ language development: Sex differences within social risk. Developmental Psychology, 31: (5), 851.

37 

Palfrey J. S. , Levine M. D. , Walker D. K. , & Sullivan M. ((1985) ). The emergence of attention deficits in early childhood: A prospective study. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics: JDBP, 6: (6), 339–348. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/4077993.

38 

Quiroga M. A. , Santacreu J. , Montoro A. , Martínez-Molina A. , & Shih P. Ch. ((2011) ). Evaluación informatizada de 7 la 11 atención para niños de a años: El DiViSA-UAM y el TACI-UAM [Computerized assessment of attention for children from 7 to 11 years old: DiViSA-UAM and TACI-UAM]. Clínica y Salud, 22: , 5–12.

39 

Rahmann A. , Stodieck S. , Husstedt I.-W. , & Evers S. ((2002) ). Pre-attentive cognitive processing in epilepsy. A pilot study on the impact of epilepsy type and anti-epileptic treatment. European Neurology, 48: (3), 146–152. https://doi.org/65517

40 

Rivera D. , & Arango-Lasprilla J. C. ((2017) ). Methodology for the development of normative data for Spanish Speaking pediatric population. NeuroRehabilitation, 41: (3), 581–592.

41 

Rosselli M. , Ardila A. , Bateman J. R. , & Guzmán M. ((2001) ). Developmental norms in the neuropsychological evaluation of Spanishs peaking children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20: (1), 355–374.

42 

Rosselli M. , Matute E. , Ardila A. , Botero V. , Tangerife G. , Echeverria S. R. , & Ocampo P. ((2004) ). ENI: Una batería neuropsicológica para la evaluación de niños con edades entre los 5 y los 16 años. Un estudio normativo colombiano. Revista de Neurologίa, 38: (8), 720–731.

43 

Santacreu J. , Shih P. , & Quiroga M. A. ((2010) ). DiViSA, test de discriminación visual simple deárboles [DiViSA, trees simple visual discrimination test]. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.

44 

Schady N. ((2011) ). Parents’ education, mothers’ vocabulary, and cognitive development in early childhood: Longitudinal evidence from Ecuador. American Journal of Public Health, 101: (12), 2299–2307.

45 

Strauss E. , Sherman E. M. , & Spreen O. ((2006) ), A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary. American Chemical Society.

46 

Sweet J. J. , Benson L. M. , Nelson N. W. , & Moberg P. J. ((2015) ). The American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology, National Academy of Neuropsychology, and Society for Clinical Neuropsychology (APA Division) 2015 TCN Professional Practice and “Salary Survey”: Professional Practices, Beliefs, and Incomes of U.S. Neur. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 29: (8), 1069–1162. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2016.1140228

47 

van den Berg V. , Saliasi E. , de Groot R. H. M. , Jolles J. , Chinapaw M. J. M. , & Singh A. S. ((2016) ). Physical activity in the school setting: Cognitive performance is not affected by three different types of acute exercise. Frontiers in Psychology. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00723

48 

Matute E. , Sanz A. , Gum E. , Rosselli M. , & Ardila A. ((2009) ). Influencia del nivel educativo de los padres, el tipo de escuela y el sexo en el desarrollo de la atención y la memoria. Revista latinoamericana de psicología, 41: (2), 257–276.

49 

Wassenberg R. , Hendriksen J. G. , Hurks P. P. , Feron F. J. , Keulers E. H. , Vles J. S. , & Jolles J. ((2008) ). Development of inattention, impulsivity, and processing speed as measured by the d2 test: Results of a large cross-sectional study in children aged. Child Neuropsychology, 14: (3), 195–210.