You are viewing a javascript disabled version of the site. Please enable Javascript for this site to function properly.
Go to headerGo to navigationGo to searchGo to contentsGo to footer
In content section. Select this link to jump to navigation

Role of non-invasive objective markers for the rehabilitative diagnosis of central sensitization in patients with fibromyalgia: A systematic review

Abstract

BACKGROUND:

Central sensitization cannot be demonstrated directly in humans. Therefore, studies used different proxy markers (signs, symptoms and tools) to identify factors assumed to relate to central sensitization in humans, that is, Human Assumed Central Sensitization (HACS). The aims of this systematic review were to identify non-invasive objective markers of HACS and the instruments to assess these markers in patients with fibromyalgia (FM).

METHODS:

A systematic review was conducted with the following inclusion criteria: (1) adults, (2) diagnosed with FM, and (3) markers and instruments for HACS had to be non-invasive. Data were subsequently extracted, and studies were assessed for risk of bias using the quality assessment tools developed by the National Institute of Health.

RESULTS:

78 studies (n= 5234 participants) were included and the findings were categorized in markers identified to assess peripheral and central manifestations of HACS. The identified markers for peripheral manifestations of HACS, with at least moderate evidence, were pain after-sensation decline rates, mechanical pain thresholds, pressure pain threshold, sound ‘pressure’ pain threshold, cutaneous silent period, slowly repeated evoked pain sensitization and nociceptive flexion reflex threshold. The identified markers for central manifestations of HACS were efficacy of conditioned pain modulation with pressure pain conditioning and brain perfusion analysis. Instruments to assess these markers are: pin-prick stimulators, cuff-algometry, repetitive pressure stimulation using a pressure algometer, sound, electrodes and neuroimaging techniques.

CONCLUSIONS:

This review provides an overview of non-invasive markers and instruments for the assessment of HACS in patients with FM. Implementing these findings into clinical settings may help to identify HACS in patients with FM.

1.Introduction

The term nociplastic pain is defined as “pain that arises from altered nociception despite no clear evidence of actual or threatened tissue damage causing the activation of peripheral nociceptors or evidence for disease or lesion of the somatosensory system causing the pai” [1]. Central sensitization (CS) can be described as “an increased responsiveness of nociceptive neurons in the central nervous system to their normal or subthreshold afferent input” and can therefore be an expression of nociplastic pain [2]. CS results in an enhanced nociceptive neural signaling, meaning that the stimulus intensity necessary to elicit the pain response is lowered, resulting in pain hypersensitivity [3]. This alteration in sensory processing systems is observed in animal experiments [4], and this phenomenon is supposed to be of value to explain multiple chronic pain conditions such as low back pain, osteoarthritis, temporomandibular disorders and fibromyalgia (FM) [5]. FM, with a worldwide prevalence of 2–4%, and temporomandibular disorders are among the most common causes of pain and disability related to CS [6]. Both disorder share features and are influenced by genetic, biological, and psychosocial factors, such as diet, obesity and stressful events [6, 7, 8]. Because there is no gold standard for the assessment of CS, the presence of it cannot be demonstrated directly in humans. Instead, studies used different proxy markers (signs, symptoms and tools) to identify factors assumed to relate to CS in human, that is, Human Assumed Central Sensitization (HACS). A proxy marker for HACS can be defined as an indirect measurable indicator of the assumed presence of CS. These proxy markers will further be referred to as ‘marker’ in this review. The term HACS has previously been defined in a review on HACS in patients with chronic low back pain [9]. As FM is related to CS, rehabilitative therapy could play a useful role in the improvement of pain-related and mobility symptoms [7]. Thus, improving the accuracy of FM diagnosis can aid in the rehabilitation process of patients with FM.

Because of the absence of a typical physiological abnormality specific to FM, the diagnosis of FM is based on clinical presentation only, with a diagnostic criterium using a list of eighteen body sites and experiencing pain in at least 11 of the 18 tender points [10]. The 1990 American College of Rheumatology (ACR) classification for diagnosis comprises the assessment of pain in eighteen body sites combined with the average scores of a self-administered questionnaire [11]. The revised 2010 ACR classification includes a calculation of the widespread pain index, a symptom severity scale and does not contain a tender point examination. Diagnostic studies in patients with FM were conducted with the aim of identifying HACS markers, varying from cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) and serum concentrations [12, 13, 14] to urinary metabolites such as creatine [11]. Simple clinical tests to objectively identify HACS markers may, however, contribute to setting more suitable and objective diagnosis which are clinically feasible. While there appear to be several studies available, an overview of the current state is missing. The aim of this study was to review the literature to determine non-invasive markers for the presence of HACS in patients with FM and the instruments needed for the assessment of these markers.

Table 1

Eligibility criteria for study selection

Inclusion criteriaExclusion criteria
Population

  • 1. Human population

  • 2. Adults (age 18 or above)

  • 1. Animals

  • 2. Children (age below 18)

Target condition

  • 3. Fibromyalgia diagnosis

  • 3. Pain due to malignancy

  • 4. Psychosocial problems part of the DSM-5 classification

Type of studies

  • 4. Cross-sectional, cohort, case-control, observational diagnostic, validation studies

  • 5. Studies published between 01/01/1994 to 01/04/2022

  • 5. Systematic reviews

  • 6. Meta-analyses

  • 7. Studies before 01/01/1994 OR after 01/04/2022

Outcomes

  • 6. Neurophysiological and non-invasive markers for central sensitization

  • 8. Use of only invasive markers (blood, urine tests)

2.Methods

The search strategy started with a broad search regarding non-invasive markers of HACS for three chronic musculoskeletal pain diagnoses: fibromyalgia, chronic low back pain and osteoarthritis. Due to the vast amount of hits provided by the search, the authors decided to split it in three parts. Therefore, the current study constitutes the first part of a larger review about pain processing in chronic musculoskeletal pain disorders and is focused on markers for HACS in patients with FM. A second part is focused on HACS in patients with chronic low back pain and a third study focusses on HACS in patients with osteoarthritis and other painful syndromes. The current systematic review was reported based on the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) [15] and has been prospectively registered in Prospero (October 2020: CRD42020172382).

2.1Search strategy

Three electronic databases (PubMed, EMBASE and PsycINFO) were searched on 01/04/2022. MeSH terms in PubMed were incorporated in the search string. Keywords were divided into the three following categories: the target population consisted of patients with “Musculoskeletal Pain” OR “Chronic Pain”. The target condition was HACS. Because there is no consensus or uniformity in terminology, we used the following search terms for HACS conditions: “Central Sensitizatio” OR “Centralized Pain”, “Hypersensitivity” and synonyms. Finally, the outcome measures “Neurophysiological Biomarker” were related to non-invasive HACS markers. Non-invasive markers are defined as markers determined through a procedure that does not cause a break in the skin, nor creates contact with the mucosa or an internal body cavity. Synonyms of the different keyword groups constituted the search request. The entire search string is presented in Appendix A.

2.2Eligibility criteria

Figure 1.

Flow diagram of study selection process. *Consider, if feasible to do so, reporting the number of records identified from each database or register searched (rather than the total number across all databases/registers). **If automation tools were used, indicate how many records were excluded by a human and how many were excluded by automation tools. From: Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 2021;372:n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71. For more information, visit: http://www.prisma-statement.org/.

Flow diagram of study selection process. *Consider, if feasible to do so, reporting the number of records identified from each database or register searched (rather than the total number across all databases/registers). **If automation tools were used, indicate how many records were excluded by a human and how many were excluded by automation tools. From: Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 2021;372:n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71. For more information, visit: http://www.prisma-statement.org/.

The eligibility criteria for the article selection are presented in Table 1. The following inclusion criteria were applied: (1) participants had to be adults (age 18 years or older; (2) patients had to be diagnosed with FM according to the American College of Rheumatology (ACR) criteria of 2010 and with the ACR criteria of 1990 for papers published before 201; (3) HACS markers had to be neurophysiological and non-invasiv; (4) the selected studies were published between 01/01/1994 and 01/04/2022. Articles were excluded if (1) included participants suffered from other forms of pain besides FM (but when patients with FM were compared to patients with other forms of pain besides FM these studies were included); (2) participants suffered from psychiatric comorbidity following specified DSM criteria; (3) the study designs were systematic reviews or meta-analyse; (4) the articles used only invasive markers such as blood tests.

2.3Study selection

The studies were screened (based on title and abstract) by three independent reviewers to exclude studies that were not specific to FM and the study aim. YS screened all, RS screened the first hal; and HT screened the second half. The reviewers subsequently selected articles for inclusion based on full text (n= 112). The reviewers discussed this list of studies to resolve disagreements. In the end, there were 78 papers included and 34 studies excluded (see Fig. 1).

2.4Data extraction

The data extraction process was performed by YS. Two researchers (RS and HT) reviewed the extracted data. The following information was extracted from each study and documented into a table: (1) the study (author names and publication date), (2) the population (number of participants with FM, number of healthy controls (HC) if applicable, age, gender and country, (3) study design, (4) aim of the study, (5) hypotheses, (6) inclusion/exclusion criteria, (7) assessment methods, (8) main findings and (9) definitions of HACS, nociplastic pain or hypersensitivity, when stated in the article.

2.5Risk of bias and quality assessment

Quality assessment of the included articles was carried out using the National Institute of Health (NIH) Quality Assessment Tool for case-control studies, observational cohort and cross-sectional studies, and randomized controlled trials (RCTs) [16]. The NIH tool consists of 13 questions for case-control studies, 14 questions for cross-sectional studies and 14 questions for RCTs. Before assessing all the articles, YS, RS and HT first assessed 6 randomly chosen articles and then discussed it together to determine whether they all deduced the same understanding of the assessment questions. Possible answers for each question of the quality assessment were “ye”, “no”, “cannot determine, not applicable or not reported”. The answer ‘ye’ gave one point, whereas the other answers gave zero points to the study. An overall score between 0 and 13 for case-control studies or 0 and 14 for cross-sectional studies and RCT’s, was then calculated for each included study and the studies were subsequently judged as “good” (score of 75% or above), “fair” (score of 50–75%) or “poor” (score below 50%) quality [17]. Discussions between the three authors were held to solve any encountered disagreements.

The quality of the studies was taken into consideration when interpreting results. Markers identified from studies with a quality of at least ‘fair’ were interpreted as more reliable markers than those identified from studies ranked as ‘poor’ quality. Furthermore, conflicting outcomes from papers studying the same potential marker were considered as inconsistent results, consequently weighing the marker as ‘not valid’.

2.6Study descriptives

The study descriptives of included articles are population (age and sex), country and number of included participants (patients and healthy controls). The results were divided into two main categories based on whether markers were detected by using measurements to assess peripheral or central manifestation of HACS.

3.Results

3.1Search and selection

A total of 78 studies fulfilled the eligibility criteria (Table 1) and were included in this study. Peripheral manifestations of HACS include quantitative sensory testing. Central manifestations of HACS include electrophysiological techniques, conditioned pain modulation, pain anticipation and catastrophizing. Contrary to the peripheral manifestation of HACS, central manifestations are measurements of the CNS, such as brain perfusion using electrophysiological techniques and imaging.

Table 2

Characteristics of included studies (n= 78)

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
Al-Mahdawi et al., 2021 [96]31 (23F) patients with FM (18–62 years) and 31 (22F) HC (17–55 years) IraqCCCompare patients with FM and HC with different electrodiagnostic testing and to see whether there is any relationship between the measuresNRInclusion: ACR, illness duration from 5 m to 10 y.  Exclusion: abnormal upper and lower limb NCSs, EMG and SSR, history of distal symmetrical paresthesia or abnormal sensory examination results, muscle disease, neuromuscular junction disorder, peripheral nerve dysfunction disordersNR

  • Distal sensory latency, distal motor latency, CV, sensory nerve APA, compound muscle APA

  • Motor unit potentials

    (MUPs) were analyzed for duration and amplitude

  • Stimulus at wrist contralateral to recording side. SSR was measured  and latency was determined

  • Stimulous on index finger and CSP was recorded with electrode on abductor pollicis brevis muscle. Thumb abduction, 20 consecutive painful electrical stimuli of 80-mA and 0.5 ms duration were applied to index finger

  • No significant difference in SSR in FM vs HC (P= 0.66)

  • No significant difference in CSP onset latency in FM vs HC (P= 0.41)

  • CSP duration > in FM vs HC (P< 0.001)

  • CSP = pause during which muscle is under constant contraction, after stimulation of cutaneous nerve

  • No correlation between

    CSP parameters and

    other ED parameters and age in FM

Baek et al., 2016 [89]24(23F) patients with FM (45.21 ± 14.38) and 24(21F) HC (48.54 ± 11.84) South KoreaCCTo compare cutaneous silent period (CPS) in FM and HC to understand pathophysiology of FMInclusion: ACR Exclusion: distal paresthesia, sensory loss, medical condition associated with peripheral neuropathyNR

  • CSP recorded by electromyographer

  • during max voluntary contraction, painful stim until complete silent period

  • CSP duration = time btw start and end of silent period (EMG activity)

  • No group difference in CSP onset latencies

  • CSP onset latency = affected by A-delta fibers instead of CNS control: if no group difference FM not related to afferent A-delta fiber dysfunction

  • CSP duration > FM vs HC (P= 0.021) supraspinal control dysfunction (previous study) dysfunction of CNS pain regulation in FM

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
Banic et al., 2004 [57]22(18F) patients with FM (mean age 47), 27(19F) whiplash patients (39) and 29(20F) HC (46) SwitzerlandCCTo show that Patients with FM and whiplash patients have spinal cord hyperexcitability which causes them to experience severe pain after low intensity nociceptive stimulationThat FM and whiplash patients have facilitated withdrawal reflex spinal cord hypersensitivityInclusion: ACR for FM Exclusion: pain for < 6 months, peripheral/central neurological dysfunctionDecreased reflex threshold indicates spinal cord hypersensitivity

  • Nociceptive withdrawal reflex to test excitability of spinal neurons

  • VAS for pain at rest

  • Single and repeated electrical stimuli on sural nerve

  • EMG reflex response

    recorded from biceps

  • Reflex threshold after

    single and repeated stimuli < in FM vs HC (P= 0.01 and P= 0.04)

  • Same for whiplash vs

    HC (P= 0.02 and P= 0.03)

    spinal cord neuron hypersensitivity to peripheral stimulation

Bendsten et al., 1997 [90]25(F) patients with FM (44.9 ± 1.5) and 25(F) HC (41.4 ± 2.6) DenmarkCCTo investigate the perception of pain in Patients with FM’ tender musclesInclusion: ACR Exclusion: < 18 years, > 65 years, other somatic/psychiatric disease, analgesics, opiates, benzo, antidepNR

  • Palpometer to check the

    pressure exerted by examiner during palpation

  • Palpation at trapezius

    (highly tender) and temporal (largely normal muscle) (reference study)

  • Both are pericranial muscles

  • 7 pressure intensities

  • Pain intensity recorded (VAS) at each intensity

  • AUC for stim-response

    curve = tenderness degree

  • Trapezius: patient’s muscle > tender than HC (P= 0.02)

  • Temporal: muscle tenderness was not different btw FM and HC

  • Pericranial musculoske- letal tissues > tender in FM than HC

  • Stim-response curve

    was linear (in FM) and

    approximately linear

    (power function) (in HC) qualitatively (not

    quantitatively) different in both groups

Blumenstiel et al., 2011 [34]21(F) patients with FM (50.6 ± 9.5), 23(F) chronic back pain (43.4 ± 8.6) and 20(F) HC (38.3 ± 7.6) GermanyCCTo disclose the similarities and differences in the pathophysiology of FM and CBPInclusion: ACR for FM Exclusion: comorbidities (neuropathy, diabetes, infections, disc hernia)NR

  • FM and CBP tested on

    most painful area on back + hand dorsum (pain-free control)

  • Mechanical detection

    threshold (MDT), mechanical pain threshold (MPT), mechanical pain sensitivity (MPS), pressure PT (PPT), cold&heat pain threshold (CPT, HPT)

  • Back: FM had < CPT, HPT, MPT, MPS, PPT vs HC (P< 0.01, < 0.05, = 0.01, < 0.01, = 0.01)

  • FM had < CPT, HPT, MPT, > MPS vs CBP (P< 0.01, < 0.05, < 0.01, < 0.01)

  • CBP had < PPT, > VDT (vibration) vs HC (P< 0.01, < 0.05) only pressure pain dif

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
peripheral sensitization Hand: FM had < MPT, MPS, PPT, CPT vs HC (P< 0.01, < 0.01, < 0.05, < 0.01)

  • FM had < MPS, MPT, PPT vs CBP (P< 0.01, < 0.01, = 0.01)

  • No difference in hand btw CBP and HC CBP has localized pain problem

  • FM had sensitivity

    for dif pain types at

    dif areas sensitivity generalized in space (sup&deep, back&hand) central disinhibition

Bosma et al., 2016 [18]20(F) patients with FM (39 ± 4.9) and 20 HC(F) (39 ± 10.2) CanadaCCTo characterize the fMRI responses in the spinal cord and brainstem that correspond with TSSP in FM compared to HC

  • No difference in TSSP-

    related brain

    response while using

    pain-sensitivity calibrate T

  • fMRI

    responses in

    spinal cord

    + brainstem, that show

    alterations in

    descending control system

Inclusion: ACR Exclusion: opioids, NSAIDsTSSP evoked at lower frequencies CS

  • Questionnaires

  • Stimulus T calibrated to subject’s TSSP sensitivity

  • TSSP condition: repetitive stim at interstimulus interval of 3 s (0.33 Hz)

  • TSSP-C: 6 s interstim interval (0.17 Hz) and unlikely to cause TSSP

  • fMRI + pain ratings during stimuli

  • T used for FM < HC (P= 0.01)

  • No difference in ratings btw FM and HC (P= 0.43) (because heat stim was calibrated)

  • No brain region with more activity in TSSP-C vs TSSP in FM

  • Dorsal horn ROI: BOLD signal changes > in TSSP vs TSSP-C in HC but no difference btw both conditions in FM

    FM have TSSP at lower freq (0.17 Hz) CS

  • Pain-after sensation > FM vs HC for both conditions (P= 0.01) altered painprocessing in TSSP-C

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
Bourke et al., 2021 [43]19 (16F) patients with FM (36), 19 (13F) patients with CFS (43) and 20 (14F) HC (34) UKCCInvestigate possible similarity of CS prevalence in patients with CFS and patients with FM compared to HC

  • Inefficient CPM and

    enhanced TS would be

    similar and

    greater in

    CFS and FM compared to

    HC

  • Correlation between the

    aformention-

    ed measures and PPT, pain intensity, fatigue and

    physical function

Inclusion: CFS diagnosis by CDC criteria, chronic and peristent fatigue as primary complaint in CFS, ACR for FM Exclusion: current psychiatric disorders, no comorbid idiopathic pain disorder, somatic syndrome or comorbid disorder of interest in CFS and FM, smoking, BMI > 30 kg/m2, use of certain medicationsCS is defined by the presence of both enhanced TS and inefficient CPM

  • Questionnaire measures (PPI, CFQ, anxiety,

    SF-36-PF)

  • QST measures: PPT, TPT (CDT, WDT, CPT, HPT) , CPM measured using PPT with tourniquet cuff, TS

  • Pain intensity rating on NRS

  • Questionnaire: PPI and fatigue (CFQ) > in FM vs HC (P< 0.01); (P< 0.01)

  • SF-36-PF < FM vs HC (P< 0.01)

  • No difference in state of anxiety between FM and HC (P= 0.08)

  • PPT < in CFS and FM vs HC (P= 0.03), no difference btw CFS and FM

  • No difference between FM/CFS and HC for CDT (P= 0.56) and WDT (P= 0.78)

  • CPT > FM vs HC (P= 0.01)

  • HPT < FM vs HC (P= 0.03)

  • TS > in FM vs HC (P< 0.001)

  • Inefficient CPM in 95% of FM cases and 0 HC cases (P< 0.01)

  • CS (based on definition) present in 95% of FM cases vs 0 HC cases (P< 0.01)

  • No correlation between CS measures and PPI, SF36-PF, CFQ or anxiety

Burgmer et al., 2012 [79]17(F) patients with FM (52.59 ± 7.95) and 17(F) HC (49.53 ± 8.87) GermanyCCDifferentiate between increased pain ratings and hyperalgesia related to peripheral or

  • FM would

    show secondary but

    no primary

    mechanical hyperalgesia vs HC

Inclusion: ACR Exclusion: psychiatric disorder, other pain origin, pain medicationNR

  • Numerical rating scale

    (NRS): intensity of pain

  • MPQ: for qualitative aspect of pain

  • Forearm incision induce primary&secondary hyperalgesia (pH&sH)

  • FM > incision-evoked pain at each timepoint (P< 0.01) vs HC

    (NRS)

  • No difference at each timepoint (P> 0.06) for pH

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
central sensitization and to correlate with cerebral activation pattern

  • FM second-

    ary hyperalgesia would correlate differently with activation of cerebral pain matrix, especially central pain inhibition areas

  • Measured at different time points (blocks) fMRI

  • No interaction effect btw pH and 2groups (P= 0.40)

  • FM > sH vs HC at each timepoint (P< 0.01) and over the time course of pain (P< 0.01) CS not PS

  • No group difference for correlation btw pH and brain activity

  • HC: correlation sH and brain activation (DLPF correlation coef R=-0.34, P= 0.01, SMC R= 0.38, P= 0.01)

  • FM: no correlation sH and DLPFC or SMCs pain transmission problem at central levels

Burgmer et al., 2009 [71]14(F) patients with FM (51 ± 7.3) and 14(F) HC (46.9 ± 6.8) GermanyCCTo investigate whether patients with FM show alterations in brain morphology in areas of the pain matrix vs HC and whether such volumetric changes are consequences of chronic painVolumetric changes will be present in brain areas related to medial pain system in FM vs HCInclusion: ACR Exclusion: other pain origin, psychiatric disordersDecreased GMV indicate CS pre-condition

  • T1-weighted MRI

  • Assessed 3 chronic pain-specific clinical markers to check for correlation with areas showing volume differences (pain duration, PDI, pain med intake duration)

  • VBM analysis: whole-

    brain technique showing

    change in gray matter

  • FM > PDI and HADS vs HC

  • FM < gray matter volumes in ACC (P= 0.01), inf frontal gyrus (P= 0.04) and amygdala (P= 0.01)

  • FM: pain duration and functional disability due to pain (PDI) not correlated w gray matter volume in areas showing grey matter volume differences volume = possibly CS pre-condition in FM

  • Pain med intake duration = positive correlation with GMV (P= 0.01) in right ACC. pain med intake duration = GMV in ACC

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
Chalaye et al., 2012 [29]10(F) patients with FM (46.7 ± 7.1), 13(F) IBS patients (37 ± 15.8) and 10(F) HC (41 ± 8.5) CanadaCCTo compare descending pain inhibition, pain sensitivity and ANS reactivity to pain in FM, IBS and HCIBS and FM share common but graded pathophysiology, both having impaired descending pain inhibition vs HC but greatest in FM. Same for ANS dysfunctionInclusion: ACR for FM, ROME II for IBS Exclusion: other medical condition, having FM + IBS, neurological problems, CVD, opioid, antidep, other pain originNR

  • Cold water arm immersion pain

  • Ascending: first fingers, wrist, elbow shoulder (endogenous pain inhibition is moderately activated at beginning)

  • Descending: opposite

    (fully activated)

  • NRS: ratings

  • ECG for ANS

  • Linear relationship

    across groups for pain intensity: FM most painful, then IBS, then HC least (P= 0.02)

  • Linear relationship (P= 0.001) for descending pain inhibition: HC&IBS felt less finger pain during descending sessions vs ascending (P= 0.007, P= 0.008) vs FM felt no dif (P= 0.44) no pain inhibition

  • Only FM HR due to finger immersion (P< 0.02) (sympathetic) and parasympathetic activity

  • Common but graded pain modulation and ANS dysfunction btw pain conditions

Cook et al., 2004 [62]9(F) patients with FM (37 ± 5) and 9(F) HC (35 ± 3) USACCTo examine the function of nociceptive system in Patients with FM using fMRI

  • FM will exhi-

    bit neural res-

    ponse in pain

    -related brain

    regions vs

    HC for nonpainful stim

  • FM > res-

    ponse in

    same areas

    vs HC for

    painful stim

  • similar res-

    ponse for

    perceptually equivalent pain

Inclusion: ACR and chronic fatigue syndrome in FM Exclusion: pain medication < 3w prior, psychiatric disorderNR

  • Ex1: responses to painful stim

  • Ex2: fMRI + painful and nonpainful stim for 5 conditions

  • Condition1: no stim

  • Cond2&5: nonpainful

    warm stim

  • Cond3&4: absolute T

    pain stim + perceptually

    equivalent pain stimulus

  • Ex1: FM > sensitive to experimental heat pain vs HC (P< 0.01)

  • Cond2&5: FM > activity in prefrontal, supplemental motor area, ACC vs HC (P< 0.01)

  • Painful stim: FM > activity in contralateral insular cortex vs HC (P< 0.01)

    FM have > activity in pain-related regions to pain and nonpainful stim

  • Perceptual eq: no group difference in brain response

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults

  • FM:

    nonpainful stim response remains

  • Both groups: self-

    reported pain correlated to cingulate cortex activity (bilat), sensory cortex (contra), inf parietal and ant insular (ipsilateral) (P< 0.01)

Craggs et al., 2012 [63]13(F) patients with FM (43.4 ± 7.5) and 11(F) HC (42.9 ± 10.3) USACCExamine the effective connectivity among TSSP-related brain regions in FM&HC and compare whether they are connected in a similar mannerInclusion: ACR Exclusion: abnormal findings, unrelated to FM, analgesic, NSAID, acetaminophen useIncreased influence of brain regions represents CS presence

  • Previous study: fMRI

    + repetitive heat pulses

    (0.33 Hz) on foot (sensitivity adjusted)

  • Current study: structural

    equation modeling (for effective connectivity)

  • 5 pain-related brain

    regions: thalamus, S1, S2, P-Ins, aMCC

  • Previous study: FM stimulus intensities to achieve same TSSP as HC + no difference in brain activity btw groups + showed brain areas with activity when TSSP evoked

  • Current study (predictions confirmed): thal direct influence on P-Ins and indirect on P-Ins via S1&S2

  • Functional connection from P-Ins to aMCC

  • New pathway found: thal aMCC S1 (in both groups and hemispheres)

  • TSSP brain activity similar to other pain activity (sensory, cognitive and affective dimensions)

De La Coba et al., 2018 [30]30(F) patients with FM (52 ± 9.57) and 27(F) HC (51.41 ± 9.94) SpainCCTo examine whether BP-related pain modulation, indexed by static and dynamic evoked pain responses, is altered in FM vs HCInclusion: ACR, HC free of pain Exclusion: CVD, neurological disorders, psychiatric/somatic disease,NR

  • Static evoked pain:

    pain threshold and tolerance

  • Dynamic evoked pain:

    slowly repeated evoked

    pain (SREP)

  • BP recorded during 5 min period before pain

  • SREP sensitization in FM but not in HC (P< 0.01)

  • +correlation btw static pain and BP in HC (P< 0.05)

    BP = pain threshold and pain tolerance

  • No correlation in FM

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults

  • Neg correlation btw SREP sensitization & FM and not in HC (P= 0.001)

  • HC: BP-related hypoalgesia (for static) but not FM

    no BP-related pain inhibition for static measures

De La Coba et al., 2017 [31]24(F) patients with FM (52.21 ± 9.59) and 24(F) HC (50.96 ± 10.27) SpainCCEvaluate degree of pain sensitization elicited by SREP vs pain threshold and tolerance in terms of associations with clinical FM pain ratings (1) and sensitivity and spec in differentiating btw FM and HC (2)(1) SREP sensitization in FM but not HC (2) FM < threshold-tolerance vs HC (3) FM: stronger association btw pain and SREP sens than with T-T (4) > sens/spec for SREP sensitization vs T-T to discriminate btw groupsInclusion: ACR, HC free of pain Exclusion: CVD, somatic/psychiatric diseaseNR

  • VAS: for pain intensity for pressure stim

  • McGill Pain Questionnaire (MPQ)

  • 9 pain stimuli at calibrated pressure level (T-T )

  • FM > SREP sensitization vs HC (P< 0.01)

  • FM < T-T vs HC (P= 0.004, P= 0.01)

  • FM: pain ratings as trials happened, not in HC (P< 0.01)

  • + correlation btw SREP sensitization and MPQ in FM (P< 0.01)

  • SREP sensitization is better for group discrimination (FM or HC) vs T-T (P= 0.01) (higher specificity)

  • No association btw SREP sensitization and T-T

De Tommaso et al., 2014 [73]199 (171F) patients with FM (40.55 ± 10.5) and 109 (89F) HC (40.32 ± 9.9) ItalyCCExamine the nociceptive pathways at the peripheral to the central level in FMInclusion: ACR Exclusion: < 8 years of education, CNS disease, drugs acting on CNS, opioidsNR

  • Laser-evoked potentials

    (LEP): pain stimulus

  • Skin biopsy

  • Normal motor&sensory nerve conduction velocities and action potential amplitudes (FM)

  • N2-P2 complex amplitude < FM vs HC (P= 0.01) but not in migraine Patients with FM

  • N2P2 habituation index (HI) > FM vs HC (all sites)

  • No correlation btw HI and amplitude

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults

  • +cor btw HI and tender point pain (P< 0.01)

  • -cor btw HI and daily life quality (P< 0.01)

  • biopsy: FM loss of epidermal nerve fibers (ENF) and Meisner corpuscles (MC)

  • Cor btw ENF density & N2P2 amp

  • No cor betw ENF &HI or clinical feature

Del Paso et al., 2021 [32]40 (37F) patients with FM (51.15 ± 6.99) and 30 (28F) HC (49 ± 9.36) SpainCCInvestigate the cardiac, vasomotor and myocardial branches of the baroreflex function in patients with FM compared to HCPatients with FM would demonstrate an inverse relationship of BRS and BEI in the 3 branches with clinical pain intensityInclusion: ACR Exclusion: cardiovascular, inflammatory, metabolic and neurological diseases, mental disordersPain in FM is defined by hypersensitivity of central nociceptive pathways and incomplete pain-inhibiting mechanisms

  • MPQ, STAI, BDI, OQS

  • SBP, IBI, SV, PEP, TPR recorded during cold pressor test and mental arithmetic task

  • Inverse correlation btw BRS and BEI with clinical pain, cold pressor pain, depression, anxiety, sleep problems and fatigue

  • cBRS and cBEI < FM vs HC in rest (P= 0.01); (P= 0.01)

  • cBRS FM vs HC during task and in FM vs HC during recovery (P=0.01)

  • vBRS during cold pressor test in FM (P=0.01)

  • cBRS and cBEI in both groups during cold pressor test

  • cBRS decreased only in HC during task (P= 0.01)

  • mBRS derived from PEP < FM vs HC at rest (P= 0.048)

  • positive correlation btw cBEI and IBI (P< 0.01) and HRV (P<0.01)

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults

  • Negative correlation btw mBEI derived from PEP and PEP (P<0.05)

  • reactivity in cBRS and cBEI during cold pressor test in FM vs HC

  • reactivity of SBP, DBP and SV in FM vs HC during cold pressor test

  • reactivity of IBI, SBP, DBP, PEP and SV variability during arithmetic task

Desmeules et al., 2014 [58]137 (92.7%F) patients with FM (50.1 ± 9) and 99 (90.9%F) HC (48.9 ± 10.8) SwiterlandCCEvaluate whether neurophysiological, psychological and genetic factors are related in FMCS observed in FM could be associated with COMT polymorphism (which is linked to COMT activity)Inclusion: ACR, HC free of pain and no CNS disorder Exclusion: analgesics (> 4 1/2 lives), specific medical disordersNFR < 27 mA indicates CS

  • QST: periph T stim: ice water immersion hand withdrawal time (latency)

  • QST: nociceptive flexion R-III reflex: CS presence

  • FM < cold&heat pain threshold vs HC (P< 0.01)

  • Cold pain tolerance (ice) < FM vs HC (shorter latency period) (P< 0.01)

  • NFR threshold < FM vs HC (P< 0.01)CS in 71% of FM (NFR < 27 mA = CS)

Desmeules et al., 2003 [53]85 (89%F) patients with FM (49 ± 9.3) and 40 (87.5%F) HC (47 ± 12.2) SwitzerlandCCDetermine whether abnormalities of peripheral and central nociceptive sensory input processing exist outside spontaneous pain areas in FM vs HC, by using QST and a neurophysiologic paradigm independent from subjective reportsInclusion: ACR, HC free of pain Exclusion: specific medical disorders, necessary analgesic use

  • NFR cut-

    off of <

    27.6 mA

    = 73%

    sens and

    80%spec for dete-

    ction of

    central

    allodynia

  • non-pain- ful DNIC

    lead to

    decreased NFR: this

    indicates CS

  • QST: periph nociceptive pathway: T perception & T PT and T PTolerance

  • QST: central nociceptive pathway: NFR(obtained after electrical stim of sural nerve area of nonspontaneous pain)

  • DNIC: conditioning pain stim leads to in NFR amplitude. Nonpainful stim should have no effect

  • QST periph: FM < cold & heat pain threshold vs HC (P< 0.01, P= 0.01)

  • Cold pain tolerance < FM vs HC (P< 0.01)

  • T detection threshold similar in groups no peripheral large and small fiber lesion in FM

  • Subjective pain threshold after electrical sural nerve stim in FM vs HC

  • QST central: NRF

    threshold < FM vs HC

    (22.7 mA)

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults

  • NFR cutoff of < 27.6 mA = 73%sens and 80% spec for detection of central allodynia in FM help chose which FMpatients can benefit from central analgesics

  • DNIC in FM lead to NFR amp when nonpainful conditioning CS and alteration in inhibitory pathways

Donadel et al., 2021 [81]22 (F) patients with FM (47.14 ± 9.49) and 19 (F) HC (34.68 ± 12.45)CCCompare the cortical activation and deactivation patterns in patients with FM and HC after 2 stimuli through the assessment of HbO and BOLD fNIRSPeak latency and HbO concentration differences before and after stimuli would be shorter and larger, respectively, in patients with FM than in HC (explaining a faster cortical response in FM)Inclusion: ACR Exclusion: pregnant participants, history of malignancy or uncompensated chronic disease, history of neuropsychiatric comorbidities, use of certain medicationCentral sensitivity syndrome defined as widespread pain and a state of high reactivity amplifying nociceptive stimuli

  • Hand immersed in water at 25C (primary stimuli) and 5C (secondary stimuli)

  • 2 min rest after both thermal tests

  • Removal of hand from water after 30 s or at first pain sensation

  • fNIRS at PFC and MC

  • GEE models to compare effect of speed of activation/max cortical activation (peak latency) and cortical deactivation based on Δ-HbO and Δ-HbO* respectively

  • BDI, STAI, BP-PCS

  • ROC analysis

  • Δ-HbO (peak latency) difference at left MC at primary stimulus vs secondary stimulus > in FM vs HC (P= 0.02) cortical activation occurs slower at left MC in FM than HC

  • Δ-HbO* at left PFC at primary stimulus vs secondary stimulus by 47.82% in FM and by 76.66% in HC (P= 0.02) cortical late response (at left PFC) is higher in HC than FM

  • Δ-HbO* at left MC at primary stim vs secondary stim more in FM than HC (P= 0.02) (table 2 shows p< 0.001?) lower deactivation at left MC in FM than HC

  • CSS score with Δ-HbO* at left PFC showed a ROC analysis with the best discriminatory profile CI 95%, 0.61–100

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
Fallon et al., 2013 (Ipsilateral) [60]19(F) patients with FM (40.01 ± 7.95) and 18(F) HC (39.23 ±7.99) UKCCEvaluate cortical activation patterns during mechanical-tactile stimulation in FM and correlate cortical activation changes with clinical symptomsPatients with FM would report subjective pain and show alterations in α and β band ERD amplitudes and that these ERD differences would correlate with symptomsInclusion: ACR Exclusion: other disorders, CNS medication, analgesics (except paracetamol)Increased ERD could be a physiological correlate of CS

  • Forearm brushing (innocuous) (mechanical brush stimulation)

  • EEG recorded

  • At the end: manual tender point scale (MPTS) examination + rated pain during palpation of points

  • Amplitude changes analyzed in α freq band: (8–13 Hz), β band: (16–30 Hz)

  • Compared event related desynchronization (ERD) during brushing btw groups

  • FM had ERD in β band in ipsilateral (right)

    hemisphere during

    brushing (but not HC)

    ipsilateral cortical activation in FM during brushing altered central processing of nonpainful stimuli in FM

  • Correlation btw MTPS scores (clinical severity) and β band ERD size in ipsilateral central-parietal region (P= 0.05) ipsilateral ERD = physiological correlate of CS

  • Beamformer analysis:

    FM activation in bilat

    insula, S1 and ipsilateral S2 cortices but HC

    only contralateral (left)

    hemisphere

Fallon et al., 2013 [91]16(F) patients with FM (38.5 ± 8.45) and 15(F) HC (39.4 ± 8.7) UKCCEvaluate whether morphological alterations to subcortical brain regions may contribute to pathophysiological mechanisms and pain in FMPatients with FM would show subcortical abnormalities in shape and volume and that the degree of these changes would correlate with severity of clinical measures (MPTS)Inclusion: ACR Exclusion: other disorders, analgesics (except paracetamol), CNS medicationNR

  • Subcortical segmentation

  • Vertex analysis: evaluate group differences in shape

  • Volumetric analysis

  • Brain MRI

  • Correlation btw total GMV and symptom severity (MTPS, BDI, FIQ)

  • mean brainstem volume of FM vs HC (P= 0.01)

  • Left lateral aspect of

    lower brainstem

    (medulla) shape alterations in FM and volume reduction

  • Correlation btw brainstem volume and MTPS scores (r=-0.45, P= 0.04)

  • FM: grey matter volume in brainstem and left precuneus and in bilateral S1 cortices

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults

  • No significant difference in mean total grey matter V (TGMV) in FM vs HC (P> 0.05) but was < in FM

  • TGMV = MTPS in FM (r=-0.63, P= 0.01)

Gentile et al., 2020 [92]38(35F) patients with FM (42.1 ± 10.1) and 21(15F) HC (32.6 ± 13.9)CCTo investigate the motor cortical metabolism and changes of LEPs parameters in patients with FM and HC during movement tasksNRInclusion: ACR, right- handed Exclusion: < 8 years of education, PNS or CNS diseases, other morbidities, history of cancer, use of drugs acting on CNS, chronic opioid therapyNR

  • SFT and FFT tasks

    repeated during laser stimulation on both moving and non-moving hands

  • fNIRS-EEF recording during tasks

  • Mean HbO2 concentrations were calculated

  • FM had slower finger tapping vs HC

  • N1 and N2P2 amplitude in FM vs HC when stimulation on right hand

  • No significant LEP parameter changes when stimulation on left hand

  • FM had tone of cortical motor area activation (and this finding was more pronounced during fast movement)

Gerdle et al., 2010 [93]27(F) patients with FM (37 ± 5) and 30(F) HC (40 ± 5) DenmarkCCInvestigate differences in neuromuscular control (differential activation = shifts in activity btw regions in a muscle) within trapezius muscle in FM vs HCNRInclusion: ACR, HC free of painNR

  • Symmetrical bilateral

    shoulder elevation

  • Different weights

  • Surface EMG recorded

  • Measured differential activity btw cranial and caudal part of muscle = EMG amplitude differences btw cranial and caudal parts

  • 0 kg, 1 kg: freq of differential activation btw cranial/caudal < FM vs HC (P< 0.04)

  • no difference btw FM and HC for 2 kg, 4 kg

  • loadmedian freq of differential activation in HC but no change in FM with load

  • 0 kg, 1 kg: duration of dif activation > FM vs HC (P< 0.03)

  • No difference btw

    groups for 2 kg, 4 kg

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
Gerhardt et al., 2016 [41]48(24F) CLP patients (59.7 ± 11.8), 29 (17F) CWP patients (55.2 ± 8.3), 90 (80F) FM (55.1 ± 9.3) and 40 (17) HC (61.6 ± 12) GermanyCC

  • To know if patient’s sensory profiles distinguish between CLP and CWP subgroups of CBP patients

  • To see to what extent CLP and CWP patients differ from Patients with FM

NRInclusion for CBP: CBP as main symptom for > 45 days. For FM: ACR, chronic back pain even if not primary symptom. HC free of pain Exclusion for CBP: pathologies of CBP (hernia), diseases affecting sensory processing, opioid useNR

  • QST: WDT, CDT, HPT, CPT, PHS, MDT, MPT (pinprick), MPS, WUR, PPT, VDT tested on painful area on back and pain-free area on had

  • Psychosocial: HADS

  • body pain diagram

  • CLP > sensitivity to PPT in back vs HC, no dif in hand

  • CWP > sensitivity to HPT, > WUR in back and > sensitivity to CPT and HPT in hand vs HC

  • FM > sensitivity to HPT, PPT, > WUR in back and > sensitivity to WDT, CPT, HPT and PPT in hand vs HC

  • Back: – no difference in back between CLP and CWP

  • FM > sensitivity to HPT, PPT, > WUR vs CLP

  • FM > sensitivity to PPT vs CWP but not in other modalities

  • Hand: – CWP and FM > sensitivity to WDT and HPT vs CLP

  • FM > sensitivity to CPT and PPT vs CLP

  • No different in hand btw CWP and FM except > sensitivity to PPT in FM vs CWP

    Conclusion

    CWP and FM: central descending control mechanism

    Anx, functional impairment, dep > in FM vs CWP

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
Goubert et al., 2017 [23]26(19F) FM (45 ± 9), 23(14F) RLBP (31 ± 10), 15(8F) mild CLBP (34 ± 10), 16(8F) severe CLBP (46 ± 14) and 21(12F) HC (38 ± 13) BelgiumCSCompare QST assessment in different LBP patient groups with FM and HC, with regard to chronicityAltered pain processing in severe CLBP but not in RLBP. Mild CLBP in between RLBP and severe CLBPInclusion: ACR for FM Exclusion: other specific diseases, antidep or analgesics (except NSAID, paracetamol)Decreased PPT indicate hypersensitivity

  • Manual pressure algometry: evaluate pressure pain threshold and TS of pain

  • Cuff algometry: evaluate

    pressure detection pain

    threshold (cPDT) and

    pressure pain tolerance

    threshold (cPTT), spatial

    summation (SS), conditioned pain modulation (CPM)

  • PPT < FM vs HC, RLBP, severe CLBP (P= 0.01, 0.03, 0.05) in quadriceps, (P= 0.01, 0.01, 0.04) in LB, (P= 0.01, 0.03, 0.04) in trapezius FM hypersensitivity

  • TS > FM vs HC, RLBP (P= 0.05, < 0.05) quad, (P= 0.02, < 0.05) trap pain faciliatation

  • cPTT < FM vs HC, RLBP (P= 0.01, 0.04) and in severe CLBP vs RLBP (P= 0.04) deep tissue hypersensitivity altered pain processing in FM and CLBP

  • No significant group difference for SS or CPM

Giesecke et al., 2004 [67]16 (12F) FM (45 ± 12), 11 (8F) CLBP (44 ± 13), 11 (4F) HC (41 ± 7) GermanyTo compare sensory testing and fMRI results between idiopa-thic CLBP patients, patients with FM and HCNRInclusion for CLBP: LBP = dominant symptom, pain for min. 12 w For FM: ACR HC free of pain and morbidities Exclusion: opioid use, pain in areas other than LB (for CLBP), other causes of painNR

  • Questionnaires: CES-D,

    STPI, SF-MPQ

  • Body pain diagram

  • Experimental pain assessed + fMRI

  • Stimuli delivered at thumbnail

  • First: stimuli given in ascending manner of intensity (start at 0.5 kg/cm2)

  • Second: stimuli given at 20-sec interval in random order

  • pain in FM (body map) vs CLBP and HC

  • tender points in FM vs CLBP and HC

  • psychological problems in FM vs HC

  • Similar pain thresholds in FM and CLBP

  • Signif. lower in FM and CLBP vs HC

  • Pressure intensity need-

    ed to evoke pain in FM and CLBP vs HC

    fMRI: – equal pressure condition: signal in contralat S1&S2, ipsilat S2, IPL and cerebellum (pain processing regions)

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
in CLBP and FM, but same stimuli caused less pain in HC and in controlat S2 only

  • Equal pain condition: 3 groups showed signal in contralat S1, S2, IPL, insula, ACC and ipsilat S2 and cerebellum. However, activation in patients vs HC

Guedj et al., 2007 [70]18(F) patients with FM (49 ± 11) and 10(F) HC (52 ± 7) FranceCCInvestigate brain processing associated with spontaneous pain in FMCerebral perfusion abnormalities would show evidence of altered cerebral processing linked to spontaneous pain in FMInclusion: ACR Exclusion: psychiatric disease, other medical condition, specific medsNR

  • 99mTc-ECD SPECT for

    neuroimaging

  • VAS pain scores

  • Hypoperfusion: bilateral frontal, ant/post cingulate and/or medial temporal lobes, left pontine tegmentum, thalamus and right putamen = affective and attentional dimension of pain

  • Hyperperfusion: right

    centroparietal lobe

    (SI, SII) = sensory

    dimension of pain

  • SPECT = tool for follow up of recovery

Hazra et al., 2020 [33]50 (42F) patients with FM (38.88 ± 10.52) and 50 (40F) HC (37.78 ± 8.56) ItalyCC (CS?)Assess and compare central sensitization and autonomic activity in patients with FM and HCCentral nervous system hyper-sensitivity in patients with FM will explain the generalized pain symptoms in FMInclusion: ACR Exclusion: psychiatric disorder, regional pain syndromes, hypothyroidism, major systemic infection, condition having an effect on ANS, disorder of cerebral vascular system, connective tissue or peripheral nerveCS assessed by increase in prefrontal cortical activity by means of fNIRS for oxygenation measures and patient history (VAS, WPI)

  • Autonomic activity (HRV with ECG, EDA) measured during rest, CPT and DBT

  • Pre-frontal cortical activity measures with fNIRS measuring cortical oxygenation HbO

  • VAS during CPT

  • HR at rest is significantly > in FM vs HC (P< 0.05)

  • HR during CPT and DBT than at rest, no sign difference btw groups

  • HRV?

  • in HbO at PFC at rest and during CPT in FM vs HC (P< 0.01 for 15 fNIRS detectors on scalp)  altered central nervous system processing

  • During CPT, FM reached peak HbO concentration faster than HC

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults

  • EDA amplitude > in FM vs HC during rest and CPT (P<0.05)

  • VAS during CPT vs rest in FM (P< 0.05)

  • VAS during CPT in FM vs HC (P<0.05)

Hurtig et al., 2001 [39]29(F) patients with FM (46y) and 21(F) HC (39y) SwedenCCInvestigate whether Patients with FM can be subgrouped regarding thermal hyperalgesia and if these subgroups differ in clinical appearanceNRInclusion: ACR, HC free of painNR

  • VAS pain scores

  • Cold & warm thresholds

    (CT, WT): sensation of

    cold or warmth perceived

  • T pain thresholds (CPT, HPT)

  • Tactile threshold: when feel touch on skin

  • HPT, CPT < FM vs HC (P< 0.01 both)

  • 2 FM subgroups (1: HPT = 44.1; CPT = 13.6) (2: HPT = 39.2; CPT = 23.5)

  • Sub2 is more deviated than HC (sub1: intermediary)

  • Sub1 vs HC: signif different in CPT (P< 0.05)

  • Sub2 vs HC: signif dif in CPT&HPT (P< 0.01)

  • Sub1&2 differed in hand pain intensity and affective hand pain (signif) (sub2 more local pain intensities) peripheral sensitization?

  • Sub2 worse than sub1 regarding sleep quality & tender point number (nonsignif)

  • tender point score chance of being in sub2 central factor involvement?

Ichesco et al., 2014 [80]18(F) patients with FM (35.8 ± 12) and 18(F) HC (32.3 ± 11.3) USACCInvestigate whether IC-CC connectivity patterns are seen in FM and whether they are related to theDifferences in IC-CC and IC-IC connectivity would be seen in FM and that it might provideInclusion: ACR, >18 y, r-handed Exclusion: treatments after consent, opioids, other pain origin, other study, psychiatric illnessNR

  • Demographics, clinical

    pain, experimental pain

    (noxious pressure stim),

    mood assessed

  • Resting state fMRI

  • IC: insular cortices

  • CC: cingulate cortices

  • FM: > connectivity btw: right AIC and right

    sup temporal gyrus; btw right MIC and right

    MIC&MPCC; right PIC and left MCC&PCC

  • HC: > connectivity btw

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
hyperalgesia in FMinsights into central neural correlates of chronic painleft AIC and r&l MFgyrus; left PIC and right SFgyrus

  • FM: PosteriorIC-

    PosteriorCC and MIC-

    MCC connectivity

    associated with PPT

Ichesco et al., 2016 [64]12(F) patients with FM (38.5 ± 12) and 15(F) HC (39.9 ± 13) USACCPerturb the central pain system with calibrated pressure pain stimuli and monitor changes in fcMRI induced by this acute painPatients with FM would display increased fcMRI in regions involved in pain processing after experimental pain vs HCInclusion: ACR, > 18 y, r-handed Exclusion: treatments after consent, opioids, other pain origin, other study, psychiatric illnessNR

  • VAS pain ratings

  • Pressure pain stimuli to thumbnail

  • Resting state analysis

  • FM > resting state connectivity vs HC after painful stimuli btw r AIC and left ACC & btw left AIC and left parahippocampus gyrus (PHG)

  • FM > connectivity

    btw thalamus and

    DMN strucutures (precuneus&PCC) vs HC

    after pain

  • thalamus-DMN connectivity related to VAS

    scores

  • IC & ACC = affective dimension of pain

Janal et al., 2016 [21]100(F) TMD- only patients (36.3 ± 17.3), 25(F)TMD + FM (43.4 ± 20.4) patients, 43(F) HC (36.7 ± 14.2) USACCDetermine whether CS is found preferentially in myofascial TMD patients that have orofacial pain as regional manifestation of FMNRInclusion: ACR Exclusion: controls with face trauma, dental treatment, facial painTSSP and pain AS indicate CS

  • QST: warm & pain thresholds (heat stim)

  • TS and AS evaluation

  • Pain threshold and TS similar btw groups

  • AS (indicator of CS) after summation trials (TS) decayed more slowly in cases vs HC (P= 0.01) but similar decay rate in TMD-only and TMD + FM (P= 0.32) no different pain maintenance in TMD with and without FM

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
Jespersen et al., 2007 [35]48(F) patients with FM (49y) and 16(F) HC (45y) DenmarkCCEvaluate the use of cuff pressure algometry (CPA) in FM and to correlate deep-tissue sensitivity assessed by CPA with other FM disease markersNRInclusion: ACR, HC free of pain Exclusion: other rheumatic disease, psychiatric disorderDecreased PPT indicate hypersensitivity

  • Tourniquet cuff on gastrocnemius muscle and subject stops inflation

  • VAS pain ratings

  • Pressure-pain tolerance and threshold

  • FM markers: isokinetic

    knee muscle strength

    (IKMS), tenderpoint count,

    myaglic score, BDI, FIQ

  • PPT and PPtolerance < in FM vs HC (P< 0.04) hyperalgesia in FM

  • PPT&tolerance and IKMS correlation (P< 0.01): PPTs associated with muscle strength tool (CPA)

  • No correlation btw CPA and tenderpoints, myalgic scores, BDI

Kosek et al., 1996 (Sensory) [37]10(F) patients with FM (42.7y) and 10(F) HC (42.3y) SwedenCCExamine whether sensory abnormalities in FM are generalized or confined to areas with spontaneous painIf FM pain is due to dysfunction of central processing of somatosensory input (not peripheral) general in pain sensitivity (not restricted to spontaneously painful areas)Inclusion: ACR, normal lab results, HC free of painNR

  • VAS pain ratings

  • QST performed on 4 sites: max pain, homologous

    contralateral site, site of

    no pain and h contralateral site

  • Von Frey filaments to asses low-threshold mechanoreceptive function

  • T sensitivity testing (CT, WT, CPT, HPT)

  • Pressure algometer

  • Light touch perception threshold < FM at max pain site vs homologous site (P< 0.05)

  • WT < FM vs HC at

    max pain site and homologous (P< 0.01) but

    not at pain free sites afferent activity modulation system dysfunction but:

  • HPT < in FM vs HC at all sites (P< 0.02)

  • CPT < in FM vs HC at all sites (P< 0.01)

  • PPT < in FM vs HC at all sites (P< 0.01)

  • PPT < max pain site vs homologous (P< 0.01)

    generalized sensitivity in FM = unrelated to spontaneous pain CNS dysfunction

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
Kosek et al., 1996 [36]14(F) patients with FM (45.6y) and 14(F) HC (36.8y) SwedenCCEvaluate influence of submaximal isometric contraction on pressure pain thresholds (PPT) in FM and HC before and after skin hypoesthesiaNRInclusion: ACR, normal lab results, HC free of painNR

  • Pressure algometry before, during and after isometric contraction of 22% MVC

  • PPTs reassessed after anesthetic cream and placebo cream

  • PPT < in FM vs HC during contraction (start P< 0.01; middle P< 0.01; end P< 0.01) and during post-contraction (P< 0.01) due to abnormal pain modulation during contraction or ischemia mechanonociceptor sensitization pain during and after exertion in FM

  • No difference in either group btw EMLA side and placebo cream side during or after contraction

  • PPT in HC after

    EMLA at rest but not in

    FM (P< 0.01) FM have deep tissue pressure pain sensibility

Lee et al., 2018 [59]10(F) patients with FM (45.7 ±11.4) USACSTo analyse resting state EEG of Patients with FM to test whether ES is a mechanism involved in the hypersensitivity of FM brainsExplosive synchronization (ES) can be a mechanism of the hypersensitivity in FM brainsInclusion: ACR, female, 18–65 age range Exclusion: current psychiatric disorder, HADS > 11, chronic infection, chronic pain causing condition, seizure, BMI > 40, analgesicsES condition represent brain hypersensitivity

  • EEG: 10 min of resting

    state + clinical pain assessment (VAS)

  • EEG network configuration for ES conditions

  • Positive correlation of

    FM with ES network

    condition (Spearmen

    correlation = 0.79, P< 0.01) FM brain

    shows ES conditions

  • positive correlation for chronic pain intensity and freq difference (ES

    condition) (Spearman

    correlation = 0.72, P< 0.05)

  • ES network has larger

    network sensitivity than the non-ES network

    (P< 0.01) ES

    condition networks are more sensitive to stimuli than non-ES network

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
Lim et al., 2015 [94]21(F) patients with FM (49.9 ± 8.7) and 21(F) HC (44.8 ± 8.2) South KoreaCCTo investigate intracortical excitability of primary somatosensory cortex (S1) and its potential role in clinical pain in Patients with FM

  • Decreased intracortical inhibition of S1 in Patients with FM

  • Higher redu-

    ctions in inhibition = increased clinical pain

Inclusion: ACR, wides- pread pain > 3 months < 10 years, pain intensity > 40 (0–100), 30–60 age range Exclusion: secondary FM, psychiatric disorder/CNS history, peripheral neuropathyNR

  • Median nerve stimulation to the wrists.

  • Assessed peak-to-peak amplitudes of N20m–P35m

  • Paired-pulse suppression (PPS) = ratio of the amplitudes of the second to first response

  • MEG

  • Linear regression analysis

  • PPS ratio for N20m–

    P35m in both hemis-

    pheres were higher in

    Patients with FM compared to HC (P= 0.01)

  • Correlation with pain: higher PPS ratio in left hemisphere was associated with higher clinical pain ratings in the sensory dimension of pain (r2 = 0.340, P= 0.01)

Loggia et al., 2014 [49]31(87.1%F) patients with FM (44.0 ± 11.9) and 14 HC (71.4%F) (44.2 ± 14.3) USACSTo show potential dysregulation in the neural circuitry related to pain experience (anticipation of pain and pain relief)NRInclusion: ACR Exclusion: HC were free of chronic pain, rheumatic disease Exclusion for both: age < 18, psychiatric, neurologic disorder, opioidsNR

  • Cuff pressure pain stimulation

  • Brain activity: blood

    oxygen level-dependent

    (BOLD) fMRI

  • Visual cues prior to cuff onset and offset (anticipation of pain/relief)

  • FM: pressure to elicit target pain rating < HC (P< 0.01)

  • FM and HC: pain anticipation brain region activation (S1 and motor cortices)

  • HC > BOLD signal during pain anticipation than FM (P< 0.05).

  • FM < responses in

    ventral tegmental area

    (dopamine-rich region

    related to reward/aver-

    sive signal processing) “altered dopaminergic neurotransmission in FM”

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
Loggia et al., 2015 [48]31(4M) patients with FM (44.0 ± 11.9) USACSTo investigate the association between catastrophizing and brain responses to pain anticipation in FM

  • individual levels of

    catastrophi-

    zing modu-

    late brain

    responses to pain anticipation in FM

  • anticipatory brain activity mediates the hyperalgesic effect of higher catastrophizing

Inclusion: ACR Exclusion: age < 18, neurological disorder history, head injury history, CVD, opioidsNR

  • fMRI and mediation analyses

  • Catastrophizing assessed using the Pain Catastrophizing Scale (PCS)

  • PCS scores negatively correlated with cuff pressure (r=-0.37, p< 0.05): less cuff pressure to elicit pain is associated with higher catastrophizing

  • Right lPFC: negative

    correlation between PCS score and brain response to anticipation

  • Mediation analyses: pain anticipatory activity of the ant/vent IPFC mediates association between catastrophizing and cuff pressure

  • Decreased pain anticipatory activity in LPFC mediates hyperalgesic effect of catastrophizing

  • Catastrophizingless activity of descending pain modulatory systems

Lopez et al., 2014 [77]35(F) patients with FM (46.55 ± 5.94) and 25(F) HC (44.64 ± 5.94) SpainCCTo identify brain response alterations to non-painful sensory stimuli (auditory, visual, tactile) and their association with clinical pain severity

  • 2 changes in FM

    1. Reduced

    response to

    non-painful stimulation in

    early sensory cortices

    2. Increased

    response in

    insula +

    areas

    involved in

    multisensory integration and affect

Inclusion: ACR, Vision, hearing normal Exclusion HC: neurologic disorders, chronic/acute pain, substance abuse, psych. illnessNR

  • Self reported measures of multisensory sensitivities (THS and AASP)

  • fMRI: alternating 30 s rest and activation(x4)

    Activation = visual and auditory stimulation + touching the tip of the thumb with other fingers

  • FM > subjective sensory sensitivity to acoustic stim. during THS (P< 0.01) and visual (P< 0.0001) and tactile (P< 0.01) of AASP.

  • FM: decreased activation in primary/secondary visual and auditory cortices

  • Higher FIQ and pain

    scores associated with

    lower activation in visual areas.

  • FIQ negatively corre-

    lated with activation in

    auditory areas

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults

  • FM: lower activation of S1 is associated with hypersensitivity to non-painful sensory stim in daily life

Lopez et al., 2017 [61]37(F) patients with FM (46.27 6 7.72) and 35(F) HC (43.86 6 6.05) USACCTo identify a neurophysiological signature sensitive to FMNRInclusion: normal vision and hearing, ACR Exclusion HC: neurologic disorders, chronic/acute pain, substance abuse, psych. illness historyincreased NPSp response indicates enhanced pain processing

  • FIQ, SF-36, HADS

  • Alternating 30 s rest and activation period (visual, aud, tactile stim)(x4)

  • + subjects touch tip of

    thumb with other fingers

    sensory and motor

    systems

  • Pressure stimulation task:

    rate pain intensity after

    fMRI(NRS)

  • Studied brain response alterations during pain processing using fMRI based neurological pain signature (NPS)

  • Logistic regression to

    combine results from the

    3 fMRI-based classifiers

    (NPS, FM-pain, and multisensory) into one signature of FM status

  • Increased pain intensity in FM for low-pressure intensity fMRI task

    (4.5 kg/cm2) (P<0.01)

  • FM + HC (for both

    intensities): NPSp

    responses (pain-specific brain regions).

  • FM response to low pressure > than HC (P= 0.01) mechanical pain hypersensitivity

  • Subjective reports (high pressure for HC and low for FM) of pain = proportional to NPSp responses

  • Mediation analysis:

    FM/HC differences

    in pain intensity =

    mediated by NPSp brain response.

  • Greater pain activation NPSn regions at low pressure = ID feature for FM

  • Higher pain-evoked activation in NPSn regions greater FIQ scores (P= 0.06)

  • Higher depressive sympt. predicted by stronger

    NPSn responses (t=

    2.09, P= 0.04).

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
Lorenz et al., 1998 [72]10(F) FM and 10 HC(F) (age- matched) GermanyCC

  • Distinguish between hyperal-

    gesia (enhan-

    ced pain sensa-

    tion) and hypervigilance (perceptual amplification of sensations)

  • Compare amp-

    litudes of laser evoked potentials (LEP) between FM and HC and differentiate between components

NRNRNR

  • CO2 laser pulses on hand + auditory stimulus

  • Verbal pain report

    triggered an auditory

    evoked potential (AEP).

  • EEG +t-tests for comparison

  • FM < pain threshold hyperalgesia

  • No group difference for sensations no hypervigilance

  • LEPs produced higher N1 and P2 amplitudes in FM

  • N1 and P2 potentials of AEPs were not different between groups.

Maestu et al., 2013 [76]9(F) patients with FM (36.1 ± 3.6) and 9(F) HC (28.4 ± 3.6) SpainCCTo characterize brain response differences when stimulation pressure is adjusted to subjective levels of pain in both groupsNRInclusion FM: ACR, diagnosis > 12 months prior to study, 18–60 age range Exclusion (FM and HC): other medical conditionsNR

  • MEG to investigate brain responses

  • Device delivered pressure pulses

  • Amount of pressure adjusted to produce similar subjective pain in both groups

  • Compared responses

    evoked by sub and

    suprathreshold stimulation (using a cluster-based

    permutation testing)

  • FM > activation vs HC

    in somatosensory,

    temporal, parietal and

    prefrontal areas at early

    (short) latencies and

    prefrontal areas at late

    (long) latencies

  • FM increased brain response after pain threshold adjustments

Maestu et al., 2013 (Reduction) [82]54(F) patients with FM: 28 simulation group and 26 sham group Age (40.7 ± 6.7) SpainRCTTo test the effect of very low-intensity transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) on FM symptomsNRInclusion FM: ACR, diagnosis > 12 months prior to study, female, 20–60 age range, blood tests results Exclusion: other interfering medical conditionNR

  • Stimulation/sham sessions 1/week for 8 weeks

  • EEG, pressure algometer for pain thresholds

  • Pain threshold increase was > for stim. group (P= 0.01) (after 1st session)

  • Improvement in the ability to perform daily activities (P= 0.03) and sleep quality (P= 0.04), and a decrease in perceived pain (P= 0.02) after week 6 for stim. group

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults

  • No significant changes for fatigue, anxiety,

    depression, severity of

    headaches or serotonin

    levels

Martinsen et al., 2014 [55]29(F) patients with FM (mean age 49.8 years, range 25–64) and 31 HC(F) (mean age 46.3 years, range 20–63) SwedenCC

  • To investigate

    distraction-induced analgesia in Patients with FM using Stroop color word task (SCWT)

  • Assess reaction

    times (RTs)

    and fMRI to

    investigate cerebral activity patterns in FM and HC during SCWT

Already known: SCWT activates dorsal ACC

  • HC:

    reciprocity

    between dACC and

    dlPFC (longer RT

    during SCWT in-

    congruent trials

    higher dACC and lower

    dlPFC activation and vice versa)

  • if Patients

    with FM have dysfunction of ACC

    reduced activation of ACC is expected during SCWT

Inclusion FM: 20–65 age range, ACR Exclusion: high BP (> 160/90 mmHg), osteoarthritis, psychiatric disorders, analgesic useNR

  • PPT assessed using pressure algometer

  • SWCT had 2 paradigms: congruent and incongruent

  • PPTs were assessed during SWCT

  • PPTs > in FM during congruent SCWT vs baseline (P< 0.05) FM have normal ability to regulate pain sensitivity while distracted

  • PPTs > in HC during congruent SCWT compared to baseline (P< 0.01)

  • FM had longer RTs vs HC ( cognitive difficulties) during incongruent (p= 0.01) and congruent (p= 0.03) SCWT cognitive difficulties are associated to less activation of caudate nucleus and hippocampus during incongruent SCWT(FM)

  • Longer RTs during incongruent compared to congruent in both groups

  • activation in caudate nucleus (HC)

  • No ACC dysfunction during SCWT in FM

Martucci et al., 2019 [65]16 patients with FM (47.13 ± 9.82) and 17 HC (48.71 ± 11.10) USACCTo observe altered frequency-depen- dent activity in spinal cord in FM using resting- state fMRI of the cervical spinal cordObserve signals indicative of increased resting-state activity (hyperactivity) within the cervical spinal cord in FMInclusion FM: ACR, symptoms present > 3 months, no other disorder causing pain, pain score > 2 Exclusion: opioid medication, depression, anxietyNR

  • Analyzed the amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations (ALFF) which is a measure of low-frequency oscillatory power in CNS, for frequencies of 0.001–0.198 HZ and frequency sub-bands

  • Mean ALFF in ventral hemicord of cervical spinal cord > FM vs HC

  • mean ALFF was observed within dorsal quadrants in FM

  • At corrected threshold of P< 0.05: small region of mean ALFF in dor-

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults

  • Questionnaires: SHS,

    PROMIS, BPI, WPI and

    SS scores

  • Pain ratings before and after fMRI (verbal)

  • 1st analyses: mean ALFF calculated for low frequencies (0.001–0.198 Hz)

  • 2nd: mean ALFF calculated for sub-band freq. (0.001–0.027; 0.027–0.073; 0.073–0.198 Hz)

  • Mean ALFF + correlations with symptom

sal quadrant C7/C6 in FM

  • Frequency sub-band

    analyses: similar pattern of mean ALFF group differences (uncorrected threshold P< 0.01). At corrected (P< 0.05), freq sub-band of 0.073–0.198 Hz revealed cluster of mean ALFF in FM at C7/C6

  • Mean ALFF values taken from regions with ALFF in FM = positively correlated with fatigue (P= 0.01)

  • No correlation with

    symptoms

  • Conclusion: unbalanced activity between ventral and dorsal cervical spinal cord in FM

Matthey et al., 2013 [83]77(F) patients with FM: 39 placebo and 38 MLN all doses SwitzerlandRCTTo assess the pharmacodynamic activity of miln- acipran (MLN), a serotonin- noradrenaline reuptake inhibitor, at spinal level on Patients with FM by using the NFR procedure and to see whether its properties affect NFR in FM ( nociceptive spinal reflex R-III (NFR) threshold is lower in FM)NRInclusion: women, > 18 years, ACR, reported baseline weekly recall pain over 40 (visual analog scale) Exclusion: CNS-active therapies, treatment with trigger point injections/anesthetics, psychiatric illness, BDI > 25NR

  • 3-week daily dose increase

  • Visit 4: fixed doses

  • Visit 5(w7): premature

    withdrawal, 1st and 2nd

    criteria assessed

  • Down-titration period

  • Pressure algometer

  • Diffuse noxious inhibitory control (DNIC) activity determined by comparing 2 NFR signals (AUC) elicited by same electrical suprathreshold stimulations. Positive response = reduction of more than 20%

  • No influence of treatment on NFR MLN has supraspinal analgesic properties

  • QST DNIC test baseline: AUC by 10.2%) low level activity and no change at w7

  • Treatment did not influence DNIC or T allodynia

  • No influence of treatment on PPT

  • MLN group: pain on the weekly-recall VAS score vs placebo. Dose-response relationship

  • Quality of life+function scores > in MLN group

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
NFR procedure = tool to evaluate excitability state of spinal neurons

  • No influence on fatigue and sleep quality

  • PGIC and PGI scores showed benefit of MLN over placebo (P= 0.04), for PGIC responders and (P= 0.20) for PGI

Mcloughlin et al., 2011 [47]16(F) patients with FM and 18(F) HC Age NR USACCTo investigate how physical activity affects brain responses to painful stimulation in FM, using fMRIHypothesized that self-reported PA and activity measured objectively (accelerometry) would be negatively related to brain activity in areas involved in sensory and affective pain dimensions, and positively related to areas involved in pain modulationInclusion HC: no chronic pain Exclusion both: high-dose anti-depressant psychiatric disorders, ACR Exclusion FM: comorbid pain disorder Prior to testing: no exercise for min 48 h, no alcohol for 24 h, no caffeine for 4 h, no smoking for 2 hNR

  • 1st visit: self-reported

    physical activity (PA) of

    past week (IPAQ)

  • Determine suprathreshold pain sensitivity

  • Wear ActiGraph GT1M to objectively measure PA

  • fMRI response to painful heat stimuli

  • Accelerometer data processed: sedentary, moderate and vigorous

  • Regression analyses for IPAQ and accelerometer

  • FM divided into ‘high’ and ‘low’ active groups

  • FM: pos correlation between responses in pain regulatory brain regions (r&l dorsolat prefrontal cortex DPFC) and self-reported PA

  • FM: neg correlation between responses in areas involved in sensory aspect of pain and self-reported PA

  • ‘High’ group: activity in left DPFC and post insula (pain reg) and activity in left postcentral gyrus (sensory) than ‘low’ FM

  • FM: IPAQ and accelero-

    meter measures were related to changes in pain intensity in scan (P< 0.05)

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
Montoya et al., 2005 [56]12(F) patients with FM (50.58 ± 6.19) and 12(F) HC (51.75 ± 5.66) SpainCCTo analyze pressure pain thresholds (PPT) and event-related potentials (ERP) elicited by emotional words in FM and HC to evaluate the possibility of a cognitive bias in Patients with FMHypothesized that Patients with FM would have increased pain sensitivity and enhanced late positive ERP components triggered by pain-related words compared to neutral onesInclusion: no medication 24 h before tests (except 4 FM). FM had tender point assessment assessment), ACR Exclusion: > 18 score on BDINR

  • BDI, STAI (mood measures)

  • PPT determined (1st assessment)

  • EEG

  • 96 words presented, ERPs recorded

  • After recording, 2nd PPT was assessed

  • Visual evoked potentials (VEPs) elicited by trials (words)

  • Amplitudes of VEP components were taken: N100, P200, N400, P300, late positive component (LPC)

  • BDI and STAI (P< 0.01 and P< 0.05) FM mood was more depressive and anxious

  • PPT in HC from 1st to 10th trial but no change in FM

  • PPT in FM from 1st assessment to the 2nd (not in HC: HC PPT decreased within assessment but was same at beginning of each assessment period) (P< 0.01)

  • P200 amplitude in FM vs HC (P= 0.08)

  • N400 (P= 0.05) and P300 (P= 0.05): pain-related words elicit more positive amplitudes than neutral words

  • Enhanced late positive slow waves in HC for pain-related words (no effect in FM)

Morris et al., 1998 [54]10(F: M ratio 7: 3) patients with FM (56.5 ± 4.3) and 10(9: 1) RA (48.1 ± 4.7) and 10 HC UKCCShow a disturbance of pain modulation in FM by using capsaicin-induced secondary hyperalgesia (CISH) as a marker of abnormal nociceptive processingNRInclusion: ACR Exclusion: drug allergy, eczema or psoriasisIncreased CISH indicates spinal cord hypersensitivity

  • Current level of pain

    (VAS), McGill Questionnaire, HAD

  • Peripheral joint tenderness assessed

  • Area of capsaicin-

    induced secondary

    hyperalgesia was in

    RA and FM vs HC

  • Area of mechanical 2nd hyperalgesia due to capsaicin was in FM vs RA + HC

  • Correlation between area of CISH and VAS pain score (P< 0.01) and joint tenderness score (P< 0.02) in FM

  • FM: area of CISH and coping catastrophizing score correlation (P< 0.01)

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
Oliva et al., [44]20 (18F) patients with FM (mean age 43) and 20 (18F) HC (mean age 35) UKCCTo analyze whether attentional analgesia was attenuated in patients with FM compared to HCPatients with FM would show a deficit in attentional analgesia and fMRI would demonstrate where the deficiency originated in the pain modulatory pathway/attentional networkInclusion: ACR, minimum 6 month diagnosis of FM Exclusion: other chronic painful conditions, pregnancy, history of psychiatric or neurological illnessNR

  • Thermal QST with thermode applied on forearm

    (WDT, HPT, CDT, CPT)

  • PPT assessment on thenar eminence

  • fMRI during thermal stimuli (calibrated per

    individual to evoke

    pain)

  • RSVP attentional task and concurrent thermal stimuli

  • Pain ratings and questionnaires (BPI, painDetect) after tests

  • Repeated 4 times

  • BPI pain ratings > in FM vs HC (P<0.01)

  • Paindetect questionnaire score > in Fm vs HC (P<0.01)

  • depression anxiety scores in FM vs HC (P<0.01)

  • scores in cognitive, avoidance, fear and anxiety sections of PASS in FM vs HC (P<0.01)

  • HPT < in FM vs HC (P=0.01)

  • CPT was at higher temperatures in FM vs HC (P= 0.001)

  • PPT < in FM vs HC (P<0.01)

  • WDT > in FM vs HC (P<0.01)

  • RSVP task performance: FM required ISI than HC to perform task at 70% of optimal (P<0.01)

  • No difference in degree of attentional analgesia btw groups: both show decreased pain score during hard task vs easy task (P=0.97)

  • fMRI showed similar activation patterns in both groups except for activation in HC in FC and ILC

  • Positive correlation btw analgesic effect of task and activity change (on fMRI, BOLD signal) in

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults

  • PAG and RVM in both groups (P< 0.05)

Passard et al., 2007 [84]30(29F, 1M) patients with FM (52.6 ± 7.9) FranceRCTTo examine the effects of unilateral repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) of the motor cortex on chronic widespread pain in Patients with FMHypothesized that rTMS of motor cortex can diminish chronic widespread pain in Patients with FMInclusion: right-handed, > 18 years, ACR, 4/10 score on mean daily pain intensity numerical scale of BPI during baseline week, complete 4 pain diaries out of 7 Exclusion: other medical condition, depression, psychiatric disorderNR

  • Self-reported pain (BPI) 1w before treatment (baseline), during treatment and until first follow up (day1 to 14), D15, D30 ± 2 and D60 ± 4

  • PPT was measured

  • Baseline: pain intensity similar in both

  • D5-D14: average pain intensity in rTMS < sham (P< 0.01)

  • D15: SF-McGill total score and sensory and affective scores < in rTMS

  • D15: PPTs (2 tender points) correlated with average pain intensity (r= 0.49, P< 0.05)

  • PPT effect did not persist on D30 and D60

  • Interference on pain with daily life improved with rTMS

  • FIQ score + fatigue in rTMS until D30

  • No effect on dep&anx

Potvin et al., 2009 [52]37 (93%F) patients with FM (50.6 ± 7.4) and 36 (81%F) HC (47.9 ± 5.3) CanadaCCInvestigate the influence of dopamine-related gene polymorphisms on thermal pain thresholds (TPT) and DNIC efficacy in FM and HCNRInclusion: ACR Exclusion: diabetes, lupus, RA, cardiac pathology, substance abuseNR

  • FM symptoms were assessed with FIQ

  • Pretest: thermode on left arm TPTs measured

  • Compare pain induced by thermode, before and after cold-pressor test (CPT) measure inhibitory effect of DNIC response

  • DNIC efficacy in FM (P= 0.04)

  • TPT in FM (P= 0.01)

Pujol et al., 2009 [78]9(F) patients with FM 47.9 ± 9.4), 9(F) HC 1 (47.2 ± 8.9) and 9(F) HC 2 (48.2 ± 5.5) SpainCCGenerate fMRI maps adjusted to brain response duration after assessing brain response to painful pressure in patients withNRInclusion: ACR, no analgesics 72 h prior to fMRI Exclusion: relevant medical or neurological disorder, substance abuse, psychiatric diseaseNR

  • HC 1: 4 kg/cm2 stim

  • HC 2: 6.8 kg/cm2 stim to

    match FM for perceived pain

  • PPT were assessed

  • During fMRI: mild pain in HC, highest in FM (P< 0.0001) and pain comparable to FM in HC 2 (P= 0.123)

  • Maps: 9 components in FM and 3 in HC were activated during stim

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
FM to show activation patterns and correlation with reported pain

  • 2 components = pain-

    related regions (somato-

    sensory and insular)

  • somatosensory component signals persisted after stim was applied

  • Insular component: FM was same as for other component: fast initial signal and duration of 18 s.

Price et al., 2002 [25]15(F) FM and 14(F) HC 21–65 years age range USARCTFirst aim: to determine whether cutaneous hyperalgesia of FMS is specific to heat-induced windup of second pain or includes other types of experimental cutaneous pain Second aim: to determine whether the enhanced windup of FMS patients can be modulated by placebo, naloxone, or fentanyl injections

  • First pain:

    lowest effect

    of placebo

    and fentanyl

  • 3 s stimuli:

    larger effect

  • Second pain:

    largest effect

Inclusion: > 18, pain-free HC, ACR Exclusion: medical condition contraindicating fentanyl or naloxone use, other study, opioid use, analgesic useNR

  • Rated 1st pain (A-fiber mediated) felt during 700 ms thermode contact

  • Individual 3 s T stimuli first peak of pain (mainly A-fiber)

  • Then, repeated 0.7 s heat tap stimuli second pain(C-fiber-mediated)

  • Repeated cold tap stimuli delayed aching cold pain

  • Mechanical visual analogue scales (M-VAS) measure pain intensity

  • Pain tests conducted at baseline and 20min after each of interventions (saline, fentanyl or naloxone)

  • FM > pain sensitivity to 3s heat test (P= 0.04) vs HC

  • FM > windup of delayed pain vs HC (heat and cold induced WU) (P= 0.01; P= 0.04)

  • drug condition effect only in FM (P= 0.03).

  • < VAS ratings for naloxone and saline conditions vs baseline (P< 0.05) but did no difference between them (P> 0.05)

  • Cold taps: < VAS ratings for saline and naloxone conditions vs baseline (P= 0.02; P= 0.04) but did no difference btw them (P> 0.05)

  • 1stpain FM: low-dose fentanyl < lower VAS ratings (P= 0.04)

  • 3s stimuli: < VAS for low-dose and high-dose fentanyl conditions vs baseline scores

  • T stim fentanyl effect (P= 0.01)

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults

  • no stat signif between fentanyl and placebo in 1st pain

  • 2ndpain FM: windup

    by placebo & naloxone endogenous pain-inhibitory mechanisms

  • FM: VAS ratings from placebo and high-dose fentanyl conditions vs baseline ratings (P= 0.02, P= 0.01)

  • cold pain response:

    pain ratings due to low/high dose fentanyl vs saline placebo (P= 0.01 and P= 0.01)

Schoen et al., 2016 [50]16(F) patients with FM (44.9 ± 9) and 14(F) HC (40.3 ± 12) USACCTo evaluate a novel method to assess CPM in HC and FM subjectNRInclusion: ACR Exclusion: medical and psychiatric comorbidities, major depression, schizophrenia, opioid, analgesicsNR

  • Thumbnail pressure

  • Cold water immersion conditioning

  • CPM magnitude was calculated

  • HC pain ratings of test stimulus decreased during conditioning with pressure (P= 0.01) and conditioning with cold water stimulation (P= 0.02)

  • No change in FM pain ratings (P> 0.27)

Staud et al., 2008 (Cutaneous) [24]14(F) FM (43.4 ± 8.5) and 19(F) HC (41.2 ± 11) USACCTo show the role of alterations in central pain sensitization and not peripheral sensitization or rating bias as responsible for TSSP differences between FM and HCHypothesized that FM would have pain thres- holds, long dur- ation heat stimuli ratings and repetitive heat pulses ratings similar to HC. But FM would require lower peak T to evoke same TSSP magnitudeInclusion: ACR Exclusion: analgesic (NSAID included), acetaminophen useNR3 tests: 1. Pain threshold to selective C-fibre stimulation 2. Long duration (30 s) to test contribution of 3 baseline T (BT) (35C, 38C, and 40C) to pain from heat pulse trains 3. TSSP trains of brief (1.5 s), heat pulses at 0.33 Hz adjusting TSSP of FM and HC

  • pain magnitude rating:

    NPS

  • somatic pain rating: VAS

  • HC: no somatic pain before and during experiments

  • FM: VAS scores 2.9 ± 1.2 before and 3.7 ± 1.4 after

  • Mean peak heat pulse T used for TSSP testing < in FM vs HC (P= 0.01)

  • TSSP was elicited in HC and FM and TSSP magnitude depended on BT (P= 0.02)

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
Staud et al., 2003 (DNIC) [27]11(F) patients with FM (52.9 ± 6.3), 22(F) HC (35.8 ± 12) and 11(M) HC (40.2 ± 16.8) USACCTest the effects of DNIC on temporal summation of second pain (previous work has shown that enhanced temporal summation of second pain is a key feature of abnormal central processing in FM + DNIC inhibits C-fiber mediated response of dorsal horn neurons more than A-delta of same neurons)NRInclusion: > 18, pain free HC, ACR Exclusion: other medical condition, other study, analgesic, antidepressantsTSSP indicates abnormal central pain processing

  • Conditioning stimuli to left hand (water immersion).

  • stimulus T adjusted for similar pain ratings in FM and HC.

  • Test stimuli to right hand

  • Also tested effects of distraction on ratings of test stimuli

  • WU > in FM-F than HC (P= 0.01)

  • HC-M: both DNIC (P= 0.02) and DNIC + distraction (P= 0.04) condition pain ratings vs baseline

  • HC-F: no signif effect for all DNIC condition (P= 0.48)

  • HC-M: greater reduction of WU with DNIC + distraction vs to DNIC only, but difference wasn’t signif (P= 0.07)

Staud et al., 2015 [85]46 patients with FM: 23 patients (21F, 2M) (46.9 ± 11.5) receiving milnacipran (MLN) 50 mg and 23 placebo group (22F,1M) (47.5 ± 12) USARCTUse novel QST protocol to characterize effects of milnacipran (which has shown analgesic effectiveness in other clinical trials of FM) on spinal pain pathways, clinical pain and mechanical/heat hyperalgesia in Patients with FMHypothesized that milnacipran would reduce clinical pain and mechanical and heat hyperalgesia in FMInclusion: > 18 years, ACR Exclusion: analgesic (NSAID also) use, other medical condition, other study, anxiolytic, antidepressant, previous treatment with MLN, signs of depressionNR

  • MLN or placebo 2/day for 6 weeks

  • QST measured during heat and muscle stimulus

  • After experimental session daily diary(pain, depression, anxiety, fatigue ratings)

  • clinical pain rating: VAS

  • experimental pain rating: eVAS

  • Clinical pain ratings of both groups during 6 w (P= 0.01)

  • But no group difference (P> 0.05)

  • Fatigue (P= 0.04) but no group difference

  • No change in depression&anxiety

  • Experimental pain ratings to mechanical stim overtime (P< 0.01) but no group difference (P> 0.05)

  • exp pain ratings to heat stimuli decreased overtime (P< 0.05) but no group difference (P> 0.05)

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
Staud et al., 2005 [46]12(F) patients with FM (48.4 ± 7.1) and 11(F) HC (45.7 ± 10.2) USACCDetermine whether central or peripheral mechanisms are predominantly involved in the abnormal pain modulation in FMHypothesized that isometric exercise would reduce experimental muscle and heat pain in HC but would have either no effect or opposite effect in FMInclusion: ACR Exclusion: analgesics (NSAID), acetaminophen useNR

  • Tested peripheral (ipsilateral to handgrip exercise) vs central (contralateral) effects of isometric exercise on pain inhibition

  • Squeeze dynamometer at 30% max voluntary contraction for 90s (MVC) = ISOM handgrip exercise

  • Mechanical pain threshold testing or thermal pain testing during handgrip

  • mVAS: pain rating

  • Before and after ISOM, HR and BP were recorded

  • MCV pain questionnaire

  • MCV: high ratings of pain, depression, anxiety

  • muscle pain ratings for both groups from beginning of exercise to end

  • Signif difference between experimental pain ratings between groups (P= 0.01)

  • HR&BP did not change signif (P> 0.05)

  • Ipsilat thermal pain rating: exp heat pain rating in HC (P= 0.01) and pain ratings in FM after 60 s and 90 s (P= 0.02 and P= 0.01)

  • Contralat: same as ipsilateral (P= 0.04), (P= 0.04 and P= 0.04)

  • Ipsilat mechanical PT: of PPTs after 30, 60, 90 s (P= 0.01, P< 0.01, P< 0.01) in HC

    Whereas decrease of PPT in FM after 30 and 90 s (P< 0.01, P= 0.01)

  • Contralat: same as ipsilat (P< 0.01, P= 0.03, P= 0.01) (P= 0.02, P= 0.02, P< 0.01)

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
Staud et al., 2004 [20]104 (96F, 8M) patients with FM (47.9 ± 11.7) and 72 (65F, 7M) HC (35.1 ± 12.3) USACCShow evidence of central sensitization in Patients with FM by maintaining windup (WU) of second pain at lower stimulus frequencies that would not produce WU when delivered aloneHypothesized that similar to WU, WU-M (maintained) would be enhanced in FM compared to HCInclusion: ACR Exclusion: analgesics (NSAID), acetaminophen useNR

  • FM tested with single 0.7 s heat tap: painless ratings (NPS < 20)

  • T was raised until painful (threshold)

  • Stim intensity used for testing WU-M and WU-AS was defined by testing FM on achieving max NPS ratings

  • Stim T of heat probe that produced maximal WU pain ratings (NPS=max 50 ± 5) < in FM

  • 2nd pain rating during WU-M(low stim freq) in FM vs HC central sensitization

  • FM pain more slowly at both freq (0.08 and 0.12 Hz) than HC

  • WU-AS (15–30 s after NPSmax) decreased more slowly for FM vs HC

  • Sustained enhanced 2nd pain at 0.08 Hz stim in FM but not HC

Staud et al., 2003 [26]12(F) patients with FM (45.9) and 24(F) HC (40.3) USACCDetermine whether temporal summation of deep muscular pain would occur in HC and would be enhanced in FMNRInclusion: ACR Exclusion: analgesics (NSAID), acetaminophen useNR

  • MCV questionnaire + VAS

  • repetitive indentation of muscle sensory testing for temporal summation (TS)

  • 15 stimuli, each 1 s long, with 3 or 5 s interstimulus intervals (ISI)

  • Psychological factors associated with FM > in FM vs HC

  • Pain threshold < in FM (P< 0.01)

  • > stimuli sensation ratings in FM vs HC (P= 0.01)

  • > rating for each muscle tap in FM vs HC (P< 0.01)

  • stimulus ratings during tap trials for FM and HC (P< 0.001) TS of pain due to repetitive muscle indentation

  • sensation intensity ratings in FM (P< 0.01)

  • Decay of aftersensations over 60 s in FM and HC

  • aftersensation in FM

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
Staud et al., 2007 [19]26(F) patients with FM (44.6 ± 15.9) and 23(F) HC (35.6 ± 14.1) USACCEvaluate the extent of CS in Patients with FM at both ends of the spinal cord by testing TSSP-M and TSSP-aftersensation of heat pain at the upper extremities and lower extremitiesHypothesized that central pain sensitivity would be not only be abnormal but widespread in Patients with FMInclusion: ACR Exclusion: analgesics (NSAID also), acetaminophen, narcotic analgesic useTSSP-M indicates CS

  • TSSP-M testing: repeated heat pain stim on hands and feet

  • M-VAS: current clinical pain rating

  • pain intensity rating and 15 s + 30 s pain aftersensations (using NPS)

  • Stimulus T to produce max TSSP pain ratings (NPSmax= 50 ± 5) < in FM vs HC at hands and feet (P= 0.01)

  • Hands: TSSP-M stim ratings > in FM vs HC for both frequencies (P< 0.01 for 0.17 Hz and 0.08 Hz)

  • During TSSP-M, FM experimental pain ratings more slowly than HC and was dependent on TSSP-M stim freq

  • TSSP-M pain ratings > and longer in FM than HC except during 0.08 Hz stimuli to feet

  • TSSP-M stim rating during 0.17 Hz > in FM than HC at hands and feet but not statistically different between 2 locations

  • TSSP-AS more slowly for FM

Staud et al., 2008 [66]13(F) patients with FM (43.4 ± 7.5) and 11(F) HC (42.9 ± 10.3) USACCCompare TSSP-related brain responses in Patients with FM and HCHypothesized that FM have increased TSSP sensitivityInclusion: ACR Exclusion: abnormal findings, analgesic (NSAID included) and acetaminophen useNR

  • Heat pulses TSSP

  • Pain magnitude rating: used NPS

  • Somatic pain and anxiety rating: used numerical scales

  • Heat pulses at 0.17 Hz and 0.33 Hz

  • Thermal stimuli adjusted to each subject’s pain sensitivity

  • fMRI

  • BDI questionnaire

  • BDI: FM had levels of depression

  • Found 19 TSSP-related brain regions common to FM and HC

  • activation of brain regions during 6-pulse condition at 0.33 Hz (P< 0.01)

  • Pain-related brain regions

  • This happened in all regions (VOI = volume of interest)

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults

  • Stimulus freq (0.33 > 0.17) and number of stimuli (6 > 2) are important determinants of VOI activation

  • Adjusted stimuli pain-related brain activation was same in both groups TSSP sensitivity in FM is not due to specific in brain activity (but general)

Staud et al., 2010 [45]34(F) patients with FM (44.6 ± 12.2) and 36(F) HC (44.7 ± 11) USACCCompare the effects of alternating exercise with rest on clinical pain and thermal/mechanical hyperalgesia in FM and HCHypothesized repeated periods of strenuous exercise would activate the endogenous pain inhibitory systems in FM and that this would be most evident during short periods of restInclusion: ACR Exclusion: use of analgesics (NSAID included) and acetaminophenNR

  • Arm exercises until exhaustion twice alternating with 15 min rest periods

  • Mechanical visual analogue scale (VAS) for pain, anxiety and fatigue rating

  • Rate level of exertion during exercises

  • VAS: rate experimental pain during mechanical and heat stimulation

  • Overall pain in both groups during exercise

  • FM pain > than HC

  • No different pain ratings between exercise periods ( rest helps)

  • During rest: pain faster for FM than HC

  • Magnitude of pain were similar during both rest periods in FM and HC (P> 0.05)

  • Sensitivity to mechanical pain in FM after each exercise and rest session

  • FM > in fatigue during rest (P= 0.01)

  • of anxiety did not differ btw groups

Staud et al., 2012 [40]36(35F, 1M) patients with FM, 23(20F, 3M) HC and 24(18F, 6M) LMP USACCTo examine how quantitative sensory tests of primary (mechanical) and secondary (thermal) hyperalgesia predict clinicalHypothesized that measures of mechanical and heat hyperalgesia would reflect relevant factors of peripheral and central painInclusion: > 18 years, pain free HC, ACR, LMP patients had to have > 3 months of localized chronic pain Exclusion: other medical condition, other study, analgesics, anxiolytics, antidepressants exceptTSSP indicates CS presence

  • Tested mechanical and heat hyperalgesia at proximal body locations (shoulders) and distal (hands)

  • Assessed negative affect (which has shown correlation with pain)

  • FM > mechanical pain rating vs other groups in shoulder (P< 0.01) and different between groups in hands (P< 0.01)

  • Heat pain rating was different between HC and FM (P< 0.02) in shoulder

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
pain intensity in patients with chronic musculoskeletal pain disordersprocessing and clinical painamitriptyline

  • FM > heat pain rating vs other groups (P= 0.01) in hands

  • LMP > heat pain ratings vs HC (P< 0.02) in hands

  • Pressure sensitivity of FM and LMP predicted 45.3% and 38% of variance in clinical pain, respectively

  • Heat pain ratings of FM and LMP predicted 16.9% and 26.8% of variance in clinical pain scores, respectively

Staud et al., 2014 [22]38(F) patients with FM (49.1 ± 16.6) and 33(F) HC (42.2 ± 12.6) USACCTo better assess individuals’ pain sensitivity by integrating 3 different WU-trains into a single WU-response function (WU-RF) which is representative of central pain sensitivity. And test whether WU, WU-RFs and WU-aftersensations (WU-AS) could predict clinical pain intensity of FMNRInclusion: > 18 years, pain free HC, ACR Exclusion: other medical condition, other study, analgesics, anxiolytics, antidepressants except amitriptyline,Steeper WU-RF slopes indice abnormal central pain sensitivity

  • Rate single 44C, 46C and 48C heat pulses of 3s duration to hand

  • Then received 6 trains of 5 repetitive heat stimuli at 0.4 Hz to same areas WU elicited

  • Experimental pain rating: NPS

  • Clinical (somatic) pain rating: VAS

  • Tender point testing and questionnaires

  • WU-AS: 15 s and 30 s after each heat stimulus train

  • WU-Δ= difference

    score between 1st and

    5th heat pulse

  • WU-Δ scores with stimulus T (P<0.01) and this was > in FM than HC (P= 0.003)

  • FM > 15 s and 30 s WU-AS ratings vs HC (all P< 0.04)

  • Decay of 30s AS slower in FM than HC (P< 0.01)

  • Slope of WU-RF was steeper in FM than HC (P< 0.003) better assessment of CS?

  • Clinical pain intensity

    was predicted by WU-AS in FM (Pearson’s r= 0.4, P< 0.04)

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
Staud et al., 2021 (Spinal) [87]14 (F) patients with FM (37.6 ± 16.0) and 16 (F) HC (48.7 ± 12.8) USACCAnalyze spinal cord activation and modulation during TSSP in patients with FM and HCNRInclusion: ACR Exclusion: major medical or neurological illness, major psychiatric disorders and any contraindications for the MRI environment, pregnancyEnhanced TSSP and pain after sensation

  • QST: heat stimuli on thenar eminence

  • Pain ratings with NRS

  • TSSP: 18 heat stimuli with 2.5sec ISI 

  • fMRI imaging

  • TSSP before and during fMRI scans

  • TSSP stimuli is

    adjusted to each

    subject’s pain threshold,

    FM having lower

    stimulus temperature

  • TSSP before fMRI: stimulus temperature for HC vs FM (P<0.01)

  • No group difference for increase in pain ratings during TSSP before fMRI (P>0.05)

  • No group difference for increase in pain ratings during TSSP during fMRI (P>0.05)

  • Similar spinal cord and brainstem BOLD activity in both groups during TSSP (sensitivity-adjusted stimuli)

  • Structural equation modeling: spinal activation observed during TSSP is associated with BOLD activity in brainstem in FM vs HC different pain modulation in FM

Staud et al., 2021 (Fibro) [98]23 (F) patients with FM (46.2 ± 12.8) and 28 (F) HC (49.6 10.7) USACCAnalyze whether patients with FM also represent hypersensitivity to sound augmentationPatients with FM are also hypersensitive to the augmentation of sound and not only to painful stimuliInclusion: ACR Exclusion: major medical or neuro- logical illness, psychiatric disease, and any known hearing abnor- malitiesNR

  • VAS ratings

  • Auditory testing with wideband noise: testing auditory thresholds and loudness sensitivity, MRS (multiple stimuli at random order)

  • QST: heat and mechanical stimuli

  • Average pain ratings in FM vs HC (P<0.01)

  • PPT and HPT > in FM vs HC (P< 0.01) for both measures

  • Sound ‘pressure’ pain threshold > in FM vs HC (P<0.01)

Truini et al., 2015 [95]20(19F, 1M) patients with FM (aged 27–62) and 15(13F, 2M) HC (aged 25–54) ItalyCCCompare the excitability in the pain matrices of Patients with FM and HC and to see whether a preceding conditioning C-fibre LEP reduced theNRInclusion: > 18 years, ACR, Exclusion: other pain sources or neurological diseasesNR

  • C-Aδ conditioning-test

    experiment studied

    changes induced by C-fibre input on the Aδ-LEP

  • Conditioning stimulus

    elicited warmth sensation (C-fibre input)

  • Following test stimulus

    elicited pin-prick sensation (Aδ input)

  • In FM: when C-fibre input was used as conditioning before Aδ-fibre mediated LEP, Aδ-LEP amplitude was attenuated

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
following Aδ LEP

  • Electrodes were used to record

Van Vliet et al., 2018 [38]34(24F, 10M) DM2 patients (54 ± 11), 28(22F, 6M) patients with FM (50 ± 11) and 33(21F, 12M) HC (53 ± 12) The NetherlandsCCTo assess pain prevalence, severity and characteristics in patients with myotonic dystrophy type 2 (DM2) and compare them with FM and HC subjectsNRInclusion: ACR Exclusion: < 18 years, other illness, depression, malignant disorder, neuropathy, recent (< 6w) major surgeryCS is less prominent in DM2 patients vs FM, which confirms the presence of CS in FM

  • McGill pain questionnaire

  • Pain catastrophizing scale

  • Anxiety and depression

  • 36-SF for health status

  • QST: measure pain and central pain processing

  • PPT using algometer

  • Determined EPTT

  • Determined conditioned pain modulation (CPM) as change in percentages in the PPT and EPTT before and after cold pressor task: positive CPM = ability to produce descending inhibitory modulation

  • Questionnaires: pain

    present in 65% of DM2, 100% of FM and 15% of HC

  • DM2 < PPT than HC (P= 0.01) and FM < PPT than DM2 (P= 0.01)

  • Electric pain thresholds (EST electrical sensation threshold, EPT and EPTT) not different between DM2 and HC but < in FM vs DM2 (P< 0.01)

  • Mechanical hyperalgesia in DM2 peripheral sensitization

  • PPT and EPT < FM vs DM2 CS is less prominent in DM2 (+ confirms CS in FM)

  • No CPM differences between groups

Vaegter et al., 2016 [51]400(263F, 137M) chronic pain patients (48 ± 12.5) DenmarkCCTo see if there are different subgroups in a cohort of patients with different chronic pain conditions and to investigate differences in pain and pain hypersensitivity between these subgroupsNRInclusion: > 18 years, chronic nonmalignant pain for >6 months Exclusion: pain primarily in genital areaNR

  • Leg cuff algometry

  • Measured: PPT, pressure pain tolerance (PTT),

    temporal summation of

    pain, CPM, heat detection threshold, heat detection threshold (HDT), heat pain threshold (HPT)

  • 4 groups made (based on TSP and CPM)

  • Group1 (n= 85): impaired CPM and facilitated TSP

  • Group2 (n= 148): impaired CPM and normal TSP

  • PPT and PTT > than before conditioning (P< 0.01)

  • TSP: VAS > after stim10 (P< 0.01)

  • Group 1: more pain areas than other 3 (P< 0.04)

  • G1 NRS scores > G4 (P= 0.05)

  • G1&2 < HPT and PTT vs G4

  • Impaired CPM and facilitated TSP biomarkers?

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults

  • group3 (n= 45): normal CPM and facilitated TSP

  • group4 (n= 122): normal CPM and normal TSP

Van Assche et al., 2020 [74]92(63F, 29M) patients with FM (49 ± 11.3) and 39(25F, 14M) HC (45 ± 12.6) BelgiumCTo estimate the prevalence of thermo-nociceptive system dysfunction using LEPs in Patients with FMHypothesized that small fibre neuropathy (SFN) is a significant contributor to the pathophysiology of FM (not supported by results)Inclusion: ACR Exclusion: < 18 years, several LEP examinations in same patient, benzodiazepines use 24 h prior to LEP recording, central or peripheral nervous system disorderNR

  • LEP recordings acquired between 2003 and 2012 (database)

  • During same time period, LEPs acquired from HC

  • No group differences in N2-P2 amplitudes btw groups (P> 0.5)

  • No loss of function of nociceptive response to Adelta-nociceptor activation in FM vs HC

  • No LEP no SFN SFN is not contributor to FM (hypothesis not supported)

Vecchio et al., 2020 [75]81 (73F) patients with FM (50 ± 10) ItalyCAnalyze the functional changes of central nociceptive pathways measured by LEP’s and the correlation with clinical characteristicsNRInclusion: ACR, age between 18–75 years Exclusion: education below 8 years and any cause of PNS or CNS diseases, psychiatric conditions other than anxiety and depression disorders according to the DSM V, active malignancies or history of cancer, use of drugs acting on the CNS and chronic opioid therapyNR

  • Nociceptive stimuli by

    laser pulses

  • Series of 30 stimuli at each stimulation site at intensity one step above threshold

  • Interval of 5 min between series

  • Nerve conduction study: analysis of sural, tibial and peroneal nerve conduction velocity and APA

  • No LEP latency or amplitude differences btw groups (patients with or without intraepidermal nerve fiber density)

  • No association between LEP and clinical characteristics

  • N2P2 habituation index of LEP at leg was altered in FM (&gt; 0.65 in 97.5% of FM, normal value being between 0.45–0.61)

Wik et al., 2006 [69]8(F) patients with FM (42–56 years) SwedenCAnalysis of PET scan measure of regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) during externally induced acute pain and rest in patients with FMNRInclusion: ACRNR

  • PET scans performed while pressure applied on arm tender point and compared to PET scans taken during rest

  • FM > rCBF during acute pain vs rest in the right and left parietal cortex and right frontal cortex

  • FM < rCBF during acute pain vs rest in left retrosplenial cortex (emotional evaluation and pain encoding acute pain the abnormally high pain signaling evaluation)

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
Wik et al., 2003 [68]8(F) patients with FM (42–56 years) and 8(F) HC (27–42 years) NorwayCSTo study the CNS in FM and compare PET scan measures of rCBF in FM and HC subjects at restNRInclusion: ACR Exclusion: organic brain disorder, somatic diseaseNR

  • Recorded PET scan measures of FM and HC at rest

  • FM > rCBF vs HC bilaterally in retrosplenial cortex (at rest) encoding of sensory events (also pain signaling) during rest

  • FM < rCBF vs HC

    in 2 left hemisphere

    clusters: one in fronto-temporal regions and

    one in tempo-parieto-occippital cortex

Wodehouse et al., 2018 [88]14(13F, 1M) patients with FM (46.7 ± 10.5) UKCSTo see whether QST detects changes in pain thresholds of Patients with FM receiving pregabalin treatmentNRInclusion: > 18 years, ACR, not taken pregabalin and no participation in cognitive behavioral therapy/pain rehabilitation or psychological supportNR

  • QST and questionnaires measured at baseline (BS) and every 4w up to 12w of treatment

  • QST static measures: PPT measured (change from pressure to pain)

  • QST dynamic measures: ischemic compression of arm used as conditioning stimulus to evoke CPM and repeat PPTs measured

  • FM < efficient CPM at baseline

  • PPTs improved from BS to 4 w (P< 0.02) and further from BS to 12 w (P< 0.01)

  • CPM efficiency improvement from BS to 4 w (P= 0.01) and maintained until 12 w (P= 0.01)

  • Numerical rating scores (NRS)

    improvement from BS to 12w

  • Improvement in PainDETECT and FIQ

  • Pregabalin increase in PPT and DNIC

  • QST measured improvement in CS and PS in treated FM

Table 2, continued

Sources (Author; Year)Population (Gender F/M + (Age mean ± SD) + countryStudy designAIMHypothesisIncl./excl. criteriaAltered pain processing or cs or hypersensitivity definitionAssessment methodResults
Zhang et al., 2015 [86]121(63F, 58M) chronic pain pt on opioid therapy (46.6 ± 9.5), 172(78F, 93M) chronic pain pt on non-opioid therapy (45.6 ± 13.4) and 129 (54F, 75M) HC (35 ± 14.2) USACSTo compare the sensitivity to experimental pain of chronic pain patients on opioid therapy vs chronic pain patients non non-opioid therapy and HC by using QSTNRInclusion: pain free HC and no opioid treatment min 6 m, pain non-opioid: stable pain condition but no opioid treatment for min 3 m, pain opioid: stable pain condition for min 3m Exclusion: sensory deficits at a QST site, interventions altering QST response, psychiatric illnessNR

  • 4 QST parameters:

    cold&warm sensation,

    cold&heat PT, cold&heat pain tolerance, TS to heat stim

  • DNIC was measured

  • Pain opioid group: <

    HPT vs non-opioid

    (P= 0.04)

  • Max tolerated heat T< in opioid vs non-opioid (P= 0.04)

  • Opioid group: < tolerance to supra-threshold heat pain stim vs non op (P= 0.02)

  • TS > in opioid vs non op (P= 0.03)

  • Lower DNIC in opioid vs non op (P= 0.03)

ACR: American college of rheumatology FM diagnosis criteria, AIC: anterior IC, ACC: anterior cingulate cortex, aMCC: anterior mid-cingulate cortex, APA: action potential amplitude, AUC: area under the curve, AS: pain after-sensation, ANS: autonomic nervous system, BP: blood-pressure, BOLD: blood-oxygen-level-dependent, BDI: Beck depression inventory, BP-PCS: Brazilian Portuguese Profile of chronic pain: screen, BPI: brief pain inventory, BRS; baroreflex sensitivity, BEI: baroreflex effectiveness, CC: cingulate cortex, C: cohort, cor: correlation, CLBP: chronic low back pain, CPM: conditioned pain modulation, CC: case control, CS: cross-sectional, CLP: chronic localized pain, CWP: chronic widespread pain, CSP: cutaneous silent period, CNS: central nervous system, CBP: chronic back pain, CLBP: chronic low back pain, CS: central-sensitization, C(D)T: cold (detection) threshold, CES-D: Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale, CPT: cold pain threshold, CDC; centers for disease control, CFQ: Chalder fatigue questionnaire, CFS: chronic fatigue syndrome, CSS: central sensitization symptoms, CSI: central sensitization inventory, DM2: myotonic dystrophy type 2, DNIC: diffuse noxious inhibitory control, dif: difference, DLPFC: dorsolateral prefrontal, DEPS: depression scale, DBP: diastolic blood pressure, DBT: deep breathing test, DSM V: diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, ES: explosive synchronization, EQ-5 L-5D: EuroQol The 5-level EQ-5D version, ED: electrodiagnostic, EDA: electrodermal activity, EPTT: electric pain tolerance threshold, EMG: electromyography, freq: frequency, FM: patients with fibromyalgia, fNIRS: functional near-infrared spectroscopy, FSS: fatigue severity scale, FP: frontopolar cortex, GMV: gray matter volume, HADS: hospital anxiety and depression scale, HbO: oxyhemoglobin, HC: healthy controls, HPT, heat pain threshold, HR: heart rate, HRV: heart rate variability, IBI: interbeat interval, ISI: interstimulus interval, IPL: inferior parietal lobule, IC: insular cortex, IPAQ: international physical activity questionnaire, IBS: irritable bowel syndrome, ILC: ipsilateral locus coeruleus, PDI: pain disability index, vs: compared to, pt: patients, y: years, w: weeks, VAS: visual analogue scale, LMP: local musculoskeletal pain, LOC: lateral occipital cortex, lPFC: lateral prefrontal cortex, MC: motor cortex, MPT: mechanical pain threshold, MPS: mechanical pain sensitivity, MVC: maximum voluntary contraction, MCV: medical college of Virginia pain questionnaire, MDT: mechanical detection threshold, MPQ: McGill pain questionnaire, MPFC: medial prefrontal cortex, MRS: multiple random staircase method, M1: primary motor cortex, NFR: nociceptive flexion reflex, NRS: numerical rating scale, NPQ: neuropathic pain questionnaire, OQS: Oviedo quality of sleep questionnaire, OFC: orbitofrontal cortices, stim: stimulation, OP: occipital pole, SF-MPQ: short-form of the McGill pain questionnaire, periph: peripheral, P-Ins: posterior insula, PPT: pressure pain threshold, PPI: present pain intensity, PPC: posterior parietal cortex, PS: peripheral sensitization, PAG; periaqueductal grey, PEP: pre-ejection period, PSQ-3: Pain and Sleep Questionnaire Three-Item, PASS: Pain anxiety symptom scales, PCS: pain catastrophizing scale, PrCG: pre-central gyrus, PCC: posterior cingulate cortex, Precun: precuneus, PL: paracentral lobule, PFC: pre-frontal cortex, QoL: quality of life, rCBF: regional cerebral blood flow, RLBP: recurrent low back pain, RCT: randomized controlled trial, ROI: region of interest, RSVP: rapid serial visual presentation, RVM: rostral ventromedial medulla, RMDQ: Roland-Morris Disability Questionnaire, ROC: receiver operator characteristics, SSR: sympathetic skin response, SREP: slowly repeated evoked pain, SPECT: single-photon emission computed tomography, signif: significant, S1&S2: primary and secondary somatosensory cortices, SMC: sensorimotor cortex, STPI: State-Trait personality Inventory (to assess anxiety), SF-36-PF: physical function subscale of the SF-36, SBP: systolic blood pressure, SPL: superior parietal lobule, STAI: State-trait anxiety inventory, SV: stroke volume, TS: temporal summation, , TSSP: temporal summation of second pain, T-T: threshold/tolerance, thal: thalamus, TMD: temporo-mandibular disorder, TPT: thermal pain threshold, cortex, TPR: total peripheral resistance, TSK: Tampa scale of kinesiophobia, VBM: voxel-based morphometry, VDT: vibration detection threshold, VGEE: generalized estimating equations, W(D)T: warm (detection) threshold, WU: wind up of pain, WU-AS: wind-up pain after-sensation, WPI: widespread pain index, +: positive, -: negative, r & l: right & left, ipsilat/contralat: ipsilateral/contralateral, Δ-HbO: difference in HbO concentration from baseline until maximum cortical amplitude of each stimuli, Δ-HbO*: difference in HbO concentration from baseline until 15s after thermal stimuli endin.

3.2Study characteristics

The study characteristics are shown in Table 2. In total, 2383 patients with FM, 1766 Healthy Controls (HC), and 1085 patients with other chronic pain conditions were included.

Peripheral manifestations of HACS were shown in the following studies: temporal summation of secondary pain (TSSP) and pain after-sensations (AS) were studied in eleven studies [18, 19, 20, 21, 22, 23, 24, 25, 26, 27, 28], the autonomic nervous system and slowly repeated evoked pain (SREP) sensitization were studied in five studies [29, 30, 31, 32, 33], quantitative sensory testing (QST) measures (heat, pressure and mechanical and sound ‘pressure’ pain thresholds) were used in thirteen studies [21, 23, 34, 35, 36, 37, 38, 39, 40, 41, 42, 43, 44] and the motor activity and FM was studied in four studies [36, 45, 46, 47].

Central manifestations of HACS were shown in the following studies: pain anticipation was studied twice [48, 49], conditioned pain modulation (CPM) was studied nine times [27, 29, 38, 43, 50, 51, 52, 53, 54], and three studies reported on the effect of distraction on pain [44, 55, 56], electrophysiological techniques were used in twenty-two studies [28, 44, 48, 49, 53, 55, 56, 57, 58, 59, 60, 61, 62, 63, 64, 65, 66, 67, 68, 69, 70, 71], laser-evoked potential (LEP) amplitudes were applied in four studies [72, 73, 74, 75], and brain region activation to stimuli and brain region connectivity were studied in sixteen studies [28, 33, 44, 60, 61, 62, 63, 64, 66, 69, 76, 77, 78, 79, 80, 81].

Table 3

Risk of bias assessment of included studies (n= 78)

Table 3a. Case control studies (n= 65)

StudyQ1Q2Q3Q4Q5Q6Q7Q8Q9Q10Q11Q12Q13Score%Quality
Al-Mahdawi et al. [96]YesYesNoNoCDYesNoNRNAYesCDNRNo4/1331%Poor
Baek et al. [89]YesNoNRNoNRNRNoNANRYesNoNRNo2/1315%Poor
Banic et al. [57]YesNoNRYesNRNoNoNANRYesYesNRYes5/1338%Poor
Bendsten et al. [90]YesNoNRNoNRYesYesNANRYesYesNRNo5/1338%Poor
Blumenstiel et al. [34]YesNoNRNoNoNoNoNoNRYesYesNRYes4/1331%Poor
Bosma et al. [18]YesNoNRNoNoNoYesNANRYesYesNRNo4/1331%Poor
Bourke et al. [43]YesNoNoNoNoNRYesNRNAYesYesNRNo4/1331%Poor
Burgmer et al. [79]YesNoNRNoNoYesYesNANRYesYesNRYes6/1346%Poor
Burgmer et al. [71]YesNoNRNoNRNoNoNANRYesYesNRYes4/1331%Poor
Chalaye et al. [29]YesNoNRNoNRYesYesNANRYesYesNRNo5/1338%Poor
Cook et al. [62]YesNoNRNoYesYesYesNANRYesYesNRNo6/1346%Poor
Craggs et al. [63]YesNoNRNoNoNoNoNANRYesYesNRNo3/1323%Poor
De la Coba et al. [30]YesNoNRNoYesYesYesNANRYesYesYesYes8/1361%Fair
De la Coba et al. [31]YesNoNRNoYesYesYesNANRYesYesNRYes7/1354%Fair
De Tommaso et al. [73]YesYesNRNoYesYesNoNANRYesYesYesYes8/1361%Fair
Desmeules et al. [58]YesNoNRNoYesYesYesNANRYesYesNRNo6/1346%Poor
Desmeules et al. [53]YesNoNRNoYesYesYesNANRYesYesNRNo6/1346%Poor
Del Paso et al. [32]NoYesNoNoNRYesYesNRNAYesYesNRNo5/1338%Poor
Donadel et al. [81]YesYesYesYesCDYesYesNRNAYesYesNRYes9/1369%Fair
Fallon et al. [60]YesNoNRNoNoYesYesNANRYesYesNRNo5/1338%Poor
Fallon et al. [91]YesNoNRNoNoYesYesNANRYesYesNRYes6/1346%Poor
Gentile et al. [92]YesNoNRNoYesYesYesNANRYesYesNRNo6/1346%Poor
Gerdle et al. [93]YesNoNRNoNoYesYesNANRYesYesNRNo5/1338%Poor
Gerhardt et al. [41]YesNoNRNoNRYesYesNANRYesYesYesYes7/1354%Fair
Giesecke et al. [67]NoNoNRNoNoYesYesNANRYesYesNRNo4/1330%Poor
Goubert et al. [23]YesNoNRNoYesYesYesNANRYesYesNRYes7/1354%Fair
Guedj et al. [70]YesNoNRNoNoNoNoNANRYesNoNRNo2/1315%Poor
Hazra et al. [33]YesNoYesNRNRYesNRNRNAYesYesNRNo5/1338%Poor
Hurtig et al. [39]YesNoNRNoYesNoYesNANRYesYesNRNo5/1338%Poor
Ichesco et al. [80]YesNoNRNoYesYesYesNANRYesYesNRNo6/1346%Poor
Ichesco et al. [64]YesNoNRNoNRYesYesNANRYesYesNRNo5/1338%Poor
Janal et al. [21]YesYesNRYesYesYesYesNANRYesYesNoNo8/1361%Fair
Jespersen et al. [35]YesNoNRNoYesYesYesNANRYesYesNRNo6/1346%Poor
Kosek et al. [37]YesNoNRNoNoNoNoNANRYesYesNRNo3/1323%Poor
Kosek et al. [36]YesNoNRNoNoNoNoNANRYesYesNRNo3/1323%Poor
Lim et al. [94]YesNoNRNoNoYesYesNANRYesYesNRYes6/1346%Poor
Loggia et al. [49]YesYesNRNoNoYesYesNANRYesYesNRYes7/1354%Fair
Lopez et al. [77]YesNoNRNoYesYesYesNANRYesYesNRNo6/1346%Poor
Lopez et al. [61]YesYesNRNoYesYesYesNANRYesYesNRYes8/1361%Fair
Lorenz et al. [72]YesNoNRNoNoCDNoNANRCDNoNRNo1/137%Poor
Maestu et al. [76]YesNoNRNoCDYesYesNANRYesYesNRNo5/1338%Poor
Martinsen et al. [55]YesNoNRNoNoNoNoNANRYesYesNRNo3/1323%Poor
Martucci et al. [65]YesNoNRNoCDYesYesNANRYesYesNRNo5/1338%Poor

Table 3a, continued

StudyQ1Q2Q3Q4Q5Q6Q7Q8Q9Q10Q11Q12Q13Score%Quality
Mcloughlin et al. [47]YesNoNRNoNoYesYesNANRYesYesNRYes6/1346%Poor
Montoya et al. [56]YesNoNRNoNoYesYesNANRYesYesNRNo5/1338%Poor
Morris et al. [54]YesNoNRNoNoYesYesNANRYesYesNRNo5/1338%Poor
Oliva et al. [44]YesNoNoNRNoYesYesNANoYesYesNRYes6/1346%Poor
Potvin et al. [52]YesNoNRNoNoNoNoNANRYesYesNRYes4/1331%Poor
Price et al. [25]YesNoNRNoNRYesYesNANRYesYesYesNo6/1346%Poor
Pujol et al. [78]YesNoNRNoYesYesYesNANRYesYesNRYes7/1354%Fair
Schoen et al. [50]YesNoNRNoNoYesYesNANRYesYesNRNo5/1338%Poor
Staud et al. [24]YesNoNRNoNoYesYesNANRYesYesNRNo5/1338%Poor
Staud et al. [27]YesNoNRNoYesYesYesNANRYesYesNRYes7/1354%Fair
Staud et al. [46]YesNoNRNoNoYesNoNANRYesYesNRNo4/1331%Poor
Staud et al. [20]YesNoNRNoNoNoNoNANRYesYesNRNo3/1323%Poor
Staud et al. [26]YesNoNRNoNoNoNoNANRYesYesNRNo3/1323%Poor
Staud et al. [19]YesNoNRNoNoNoNoNANRYesYesNRNo3/1323%Poor
Staud et al. [66]YesNoNRNoNoNoNoNANRYesYesNRNo3/1323%Poor
Staud et al. [45]YesNoNRNoCDNoNoNANRYesYesNRNo3/1323%Poor
Staud et al. [40]YesNoNRNoNoYesYesNANRYesYesNRYes6/1346%Poor
Staud et al. [22]YesNoNRYesCDYesYesNANRYesYesNRNo6/1346%Poor
Staud et al. [28]YesNoNRNRYesYesYesNANoYesYesNRYes7/1354%Fair
Staud et al. [42]YesNoCDNRYesYesYesNANoYesYesNRYes7/1354%Fair
Truini et al. [95]YesNoNRNoCDYesYesNANRYesYesNRNo5/1338%Poor
Van Vliet et al. [38]YesNoNRNoNoYesYesNANRYesYesNRYes6/1346%Poor

Q: question, NR: not reported, NA: not applicable, CD: cannot determine. The quality of included studies was assessed using the National Institute of Health (NIH) Quality Assessment Tool for Case Control Studies (https: //www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health-topics/study-quality-assessment-tools). Q1: Was the research question or objective in this paper clearly stated and appropriate? Q2: Was the study population clearly specified and defined? Q3: Target population and case representation, Q4: Did the authors include a sample size justification? Q5: Were controls selected or recruited from the same or similar population that gave rise to the cases (including the same timeframe)? Q6: Were the definitions, inclusion and exclusion criteria, algorithms or processes used to identify or select cases and controls valid, reliable, and implemented consistently across all study participants? Q7: Were the cases clearly defined and differentiated from controls? Q8: If less than 100 percent of eligible cases and/or controls were selected for the study, were the cases and/or controls randomly selected from those eligible? Q9: Was there use of concurrent controls? Q10: Were the investigators able to confirm that the exposure/risk occurred prior to the development of the condition or event that defined a participant as a case? Q11: Were the measures of exposure/risk clearly defined, valid, reliable, and implemented consistently (including the same time period) across all study participants? Q12: Were the assessors of exposure/risk blinded to the case or control status of participants? Q13: Were key potential confounding variables measured and adjusted statistically in the analyses? If matching was used, did the investigators account for matching during study analysis?

Table 3b. Cross-sectional studies (n= 9)

StudyQ1Q2Q3Q4Q5Q6Q7Q8Q9Q10Q11Q12Q13Q14Score%Quality
Lee et al. [59]YesNoYesYesNoNoNoYesYesNoYesNRNANo6/1443%Poor
Loggia et al. [48]YesYesYesNoNoNoNoNoYesNoYesNRNAYes6/1443%Poor
Vaegter et al. [51]YesYesNRYesNRNoNoNAYesYesYesNRNAYes7/1450%Fair
Van Assche et al. [74]YesYesNoYesNoNoNoNoYesNoYesNRNAYes6/1443%Poor
Vecchio et al. [75]NoYesYesNRCDYesNoYesNoYesNRYesYesYes8/1457%Fair
Wik et al. [69]YesNoNRYesNoNoNoNoYesNoYesNRNANo4/1429%Poor
Wik et al. [68]YesNoNRYesNoNoNoNAYesNoYesNRNANo4/1429%Poor
Wodehouse et al. [88]YesNoYesCDNoNoNoNoYesYesYesNRNANo5/1436%Poor
Zhang et al. [86]YesNoYesYesNoNoNoYesYesYesYesYesNANo8/1457%Fair

Q: question, NR: not reported, NA: not applicable. The quality of included studies was assessed using the National Institute of Health (NIH) Quality Assessment Tool for Observational Cohort and Cross-Sectional Studies (https: //www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health-topics/study-quality-assessment-tools). Q1: Was the research question or objective in this paper clearly stated? Q2: Was the study population clearly specified and defined? Q3: Was the participation rate of eligible persons at least 50%? Q4: Were all the subjects selected or recruited from the same or similar populations (including the same time period)? Were inclusion and exclusion criteria for being in the study prespecified and applied uniformly to all participants? Q5: Was a sample size justification, power description, or variance and effect estimates provided? Q6: For the analyses in this paper, were the exposure(s) of interest measured prior to the outcome(s) being measured? Q7: Was the timeframe sufficient so that one could reasonably expect to see an association between exposure and outcome if it existed? Q8: For exposures that can vary in amount or level, did the study examine different levels of the exposure as related to the outcome (e.g., categories of exposure, or exposure measured as continuous variable)? Q9: Were the exposure measures (independent variables) clearly defined, valid, reliable, and implemented consistently across all study participants? Q10: Was the exposure(s) assessed more than once over time? Q11: Were the outcome measures (dependent variables) clearly defined, valid, reliable, and implemented consistently across all study participants? Q12: Were the outcome assessors blinded to the exposure status of participants? Q13: Was loss to follow-up after baseline 20% or less? Q14: Were key potential confounding variables measured and adjusted statistically for their impact on the relationship between exposure(s) and outcome(s)?

Table 3c. RCT studies (n= 4)

StudyQ1Q2Q3Q4Q5Q6Q7Q8Q9Q10Q11Q12Q13Q14Score%Quality
Maestu et al. [82]YesYesYesYesYesYesYesYesYesYesYesNoYesYes13/1493%Good
Matthey et al. [83]YesYesYesYesYesYesYesYesYesYesYesYesNoYes13/1493%Good
Passard et al. [84]YesYesYesYesYesYesYesYesYesYesYesNoYesYes13/1493%Good
Staud et al. [85]YesYesYesYesYesYesNoYesYesYesYesYesYesYes13/1493%Good

The quality of included studies was assessed using the National Institute of Health (NIH) Quality Assessment Tool for Controlled Intervention Studies (https: //www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health-topics/study-quality-assessment-tools). Q1: Was the study described as randomized, a randomized trial, a randomized clinical trial, or an RCT? Q2: Was the method of randomization adequate (i.e., use of randomly generated assignment)? Q3: Was the treatment allocation concealed (so that assignments could not be predicted)? Q4: Were study participants and providers blinded to treatment group assignment? Q5: Were the people assessing the outcomes blinded to the participants’ group assignments? Q6: Were the groups similar at baseline on important characteristics that could affect outcomes (e.g., demographics, risk factors, comorbid conditions)? Q7 Was the overall drop-out rate from the study at endpoint 20% or lower of the number allocated to treatment? Q8: Was the differential drop-out rate (between treatment groups) at endpoint 15 percentage points or lower? Q9: Was there high adherence to the intervention protocols for each treatment group? Q10: Were other interventions avoided or similar in the groups (e.g., similar background treatments)? Q11: Were outcomes assessed using valid and reliable measures, implemented consistently across all study participants? Q12: Did the authors report that the sample size was sufficiently large to be able to detect a difference in the main outcome between groups with at least 80% power? Q13: Were outcomes reported or subgroups analyzed prespecified (i.e., identified before analyses were conducted)? Q14: Were all randomized participants analyzed in the group to which they were originally assigned, i.e., did they use an intention-to-treat analysis?

3.3Risk of bias and quality assessment

The average risk of bias assessment scores was 39% for case control studies (n= 65, Table 3a), 43%, for cross-sectional studies (n= 9, Table 3b), and 93% for randomized controlled trials (RCTs, n= 4, Table 3c). The higher the assessment score, the lower the risk of bias. All four RCTs were ranked good quality [82, 83, 84, 85], 16 studies were ranked fair quality [21, 23, 27, 30, 31, 41, 42, 49, 51, 61, 73, 75, 78, 81, 86, 87] and 58 studies were ranked poor quality [18,19,20,22,24,25,26,29, 32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,43,44,45,46,47,48,50,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66,67,68,69,70,71,72,74,76,77,79,80,88,89,90,91,92,93,94,95,96].

3.4Measurements to assess peripheral manifestations of HACS

3.4.1Quantitative sensory testing (QST)

Table 4

Summary of temporal summation of second pain, pain after-sensation and pressure pain threshold findings

TSSPStimulus frequency eliciting TSSPAfter sensationsRate of WU after sensation declinePressure pain threshold
Patients with FM [18, 20, 24] Poor quality studiesP< 0.04 [18, 26] Poor quality studies[19, 20, 21] Janal et al. [21]: fair quality[36, 43, 44, 98]
Healthy controls

TSSP: temporal summation of second pain, WU: wind-up of pain, FM: fibromyalgia, : increased compared to other group, : decreased compared to other group. References [18, 10, 20, 21, 22, 23, 24, 25, 26, 27, 41, 43, 44, 87, 98].

Table 5

Summary of electrophysiological technique findings in patients with fibromyalgia

EMGEEGfMRIPETVBMSPECTfNIRS
NFR thresholdExplosive synchronization conditions in resting state EEG mean ALFF in dorsal column pathway mean ALFF in spinothalamic tract region signal in contralateral S1&S2, ipsilateral S2, IPL, cerebellum Similar activation patterns in both groups during sensitivity- calibrated stimuli rCBF in retro splenial cortex at rest rCBF in fronto-temporal and tempo-parieto-occipital cortex GMV in ACC, inferior frontal gyrus, amygdala brainstem and left precuneus GMV GMV in bilateral S1Hypoperfusion: bilateral frontal, ant/post cingulate, med temporal cerebellar cortices Hyperperfusion: S1 & S2Δ-HbO between 2 stimuli at MC

FM: fibromyalgia, : higher compared to healthy controls, : lower compared to healthy controls, NFR: nociceptive flexion reflex, ALFF: amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations, rCBF: regional cerebral blood flow, GMV: gray matter volume, ACC: anterior cingulate cortex, S1 & S2: primary and secondary somatosensory cortices. EMG: electromyography, EEG: electroencephalography, fMRI: functional magnetic resonance imaging, fNIRS: Functional near-infrared spectroscopy, MC: motor cortex, PET: positron emission tomography, VBM: voxel-based morphometry, SPECT: single-photon emission computed tomography. References [44, 57, 59, 65, 67, 68, 70, 71, 81, 87, 91, 93].

Temporal summation of second pain (TSSP) and after-sensations (AS): TSSP, also known as windup (WU), is a process evoked by repetitive harmless stimuli supposed to cause higher excitability of the dorsal horn neurons, mediated by C nociceptive fibers. Pressure algometry was used in three studies [23, 26, 43] and thermodes were used in seven studies [18, 19, 20, 21, 25, 27, 28] for pressure and heat pain stimulation, respectively, to measure TSSP. Higher TSSP ratings [19], no difference in TSSP ratings [28], higher pain AS [18, 26], slower rate of AS decline [19, 20, 21] and lower stimulus temperature and frequency needed to elicit TSSP in patients [18, 24, 66] were demonstrated and are, according to our division, signs associated with HACS. The authors of eight studies showed higher TSSP sensitivity in patients with FM [19, 20, 23, 25, 26, 27, 28, 43] compared to HC. No TSSP difference between groups was reported in two studies [18, 21]. Results are shown in Table 4.

Pain thresholds: Statistically significant lower pressure pain thresholds (PPT) [23, 34, 35, 36, 37, 41, 42, 43, 44], heat pain thresholds (HPT) [34, 41, 42, 43, 44], cold pain thresholds (CPT) [34, 37, 39, 43, 44], mechanical pain thresholds (MPT) [34] and electrical pain thresholds (EPT) [38] and sound ‘pressure’ threshold [42] in patients with FM compared to HC were demonstrated. Janal and co-workers observed no statistically significant differences in thermal pain thresholds between patients with temporo-mandibular joint disorder with FM and patients with temporo-mandibular joint disorder without FM [21]. Patients with FM displayed higher warmth detection thresholds (WDT) compared to HC [44]. Bourke and colleagues however showed no difference for WDT and cold detection threshold (CDT) between groups [43].

3.4.2Other peripheral measurements

Other markers of HACS include slowly repeated evoked pain (SREP) sensitization, the autonomic nervous system (ANS) response to pain measured with an electrocardiogram, electromyography and measures of the cardiovascular system [32], attentional task performance with concurrent pain stimuli [44], the relation between pain perception and motor activity and cutaneous silent period [96]. Patients showed SREP sensitization, measured as higher difference in VAS pain ratings between the last and first pain stimulation trials [30, 31] compared to HC. The stimulation trials consisted of a series of nine low-intensity pressure stimuli with 30 second interstimulus intervals. Patients also showed a positive correlation between clinical pain and SREP sensitization, and a lower pain threshold and pain tolerance compared to HC [31]. Patients demonstrated the need for longer interstimulus intervals (ISI) in order to perform an attentional rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) task at 70% of optimal, compared to HC [44]. Furthermore, studies conducted with ECG on the ANS have shown that the abnormal ANS response to cold pressor tests in patients with FM is caused by lower effectiveness of the baroreflex responses, a homeostatic mechanism that helps to minimize considerable variations in blood pressure [29]. This was also demonstrated by lower baroreflex sensitivity (BRS) and baroreflex effectiveness (BEI) in patients with FM compared to HC during rest, as well as during the cold pressor test. Additionally, there was a positive correlation between BEI and heart rate variability (HRV). A negative correlation between BRS and BEI with cold pressor pain was also found, as well as between BEI and the pre-ejection period of the heart, the latter representing measures of the sympathetic influences on myocardial contractility. Furthermore, a reduced reactivity of blood pressure and cardiac stroke volume was demonstrated in patients compared to HC during the cold pressor test [32]. In contrast, one study showed no significant group difference in heart rate increase during cold pressor test but did underline a significantly higher heart rate in patients with FM compared to HC at rest [33]. Finally, one study demonstrated a negative correlation between motor activity and pain intensity [45]. There was a positive correlation between patients with FM who reported participating in regular physical activities and activity in pain regulatory regions of the brain (dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, posterior insula) during painful stimulation [47]. On the contrary, lower PPT and higher pain ratings to pain stimulation during handgrip exercise was observed in patients with FM compared to HC [36]. Furthermore, a lower nociceptive withdrawal reflex threshold after stimuli in patients with FM compared to HC was shown in three studies [53, 57, 58]. Finally, a longer cutaneous silent period after painful electrical stimuli was measured in patients with FM compared to HC [96]. These findings are shown in Table 5 as well.

3.5Measurements to assess CNS manifestations of HAC

3.5.1Electroencephalogram (EEG)

Various electrophysiological techniques were used to assess HACS. Resting EEG measurements have shown a positive correlation between having FM and explosive synchronization conditions in patients with FM [59] (explosive synchronization being a condition in which a small perturbation leads to global propagation [59]). Furthermore, EEG has been helpful in demonstrating higher [72], lower [73] and identical [74, 75] amplitudes of the N1 and P2 components of laser-evoked potentials (LEP) in patients with FM compared to HC [72]. One study analyzed the biphasic N2P2 component of LEP’s and found an altered N2P2 habituation index in patients with FM [75] (habituation index representing whether or not subjects showed a decreased or increased response to stimuli repetition).

3.5.2Brain activity and perfusion

Furthermore, fMRI was used to demonstrate higher mean amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations in the ventral hemicord, which turned out to be decreased in the dorsal quadrants of patient’ cervical spinal cord compared to HC [65]. Higher activation in fMRI-based neurologic pain signature regions was observed in patients with FM compared to HC during painful stimulation [61], indicating that this region is hyperactive in patients, suggesting to be an expression of HACS in patients with FM. fMRI also showed higher brain activity in different brain regions during identical pressure stimulation in patients with FM compared to HC [67]. When the stimulation intensity was adapted to create subjectively equal pain intensity for subjects in both groups, patients and HC showed similar fMRI activity [67]. This suggests that hyperactive regions can be an expression of HACS in patients with FM, as it was not activated in HC during identical pressure stimulation fMRI measures during TSSP, elicited by heat stimuli adjusted to individual’s pain threshold, also showed higher blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) activation patterns in the spinal cord of patients with and without FM. This activation also seemed to be associated with increased BOLD activity in the brainstem of patients with FM compared to HC [28]. Another study also demonstrated similar activation patterns in both groups after sensitivity-adjusted thermal stimuli, except for an increased activation of two brain regions in HC compared to patients with FM [44]. Furthermore, there was a positive correlation between the analgesic effect of the task and the BOLD activity detected on fMRI in both groups [44]. Brain perfusion analysis offered promising results using PET and SPECT neuroimaging. PET scan analysis showed increased regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) bilaterally in the retrosplenial cortex (area that encodes sensory events, pain included) at rest in patients with FM compared to HC [68], increased rCBF in the parietal cortex and decreased rCBF in the retrosplenial cortex during painful stimulation compared to rest was also observed with PET scans in patients with FM [69], hyperperfusion in S1 and S2 areas of patients with FM was demonstrated using SPECT neuroimaging [70]. Functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) measurements at the motor cortex (MO) showed greater hemoglobin-oxygen (HbO) concentration differences between two consecutive thermal stimuli in patients with FM compared to HC, suggesting a slower rate of cortical activation in the motor cortex of patients with FM [81]. In contrast, another study demonstrated a higher increase in HbO concentration in the left PFC between rest and cold pressor test in patients with FM compared to HC [33]. During CPT, patients with FM reached peak HbO concentrations faster than HC [33] and also demonstrated greater electrodermal activity amplitudes than HC [33].

3.5.3Gray matter volume alterations

As final electrophysiological technique, voxel-based morphometry (VBM) analysis, showing gray matter volume alterations, yielded different results between FM patients and HC. Patients with FM presented with decreased grey matter volume in the anterior cingulate cortex [71] and increased grey matter volume in S1 bilaterally, compared to HC [60]. It was demonstrated that VBM-detected gray matter volume alterations in the anterior cingulate cortex are associated to HACS [71]. In contrast, the anterior cingulate cortex and amygdala volumetric changes are not associated with pain duration or functional disability. This suggests that these volumetric differences are not consequences of FM but could rather be a pre-condition for HACS development in FM [71], potentially making voxel-based morphometry a marker for HACS assessment. Additional results are displayed in Table 5.

Table 6

Overview of the identified markers

HACS markersTools
Peripheral manifestations of HACS
 Pain after sensations and decline ratesNumerical pain scale (NPS)
 Mechanical pain thresholdPin prick stimulators
 Pressure pain thresholdPressure algometry
 Sound ‘pressure’ pain threshold*Wideband noise auditory testing
 Autonomic nervous system response to painElectrocardiography
 SREP sensitization*Pressure stimuli
 Cutaneous silent periodElectrode
 Nociceptive flexion reflexElectromyography
Central manifestations of HACS
 Explosive synchronization networksEEG
 Brain activity variations (ALFF, neurologic pain signature response)*fMRI
 Brain perfusion differences*PET, SPECT scans, fNIRS and fMRI
 Gray matter volume changesVoxel based morphometry
 Conditioned pain modulation*Tourniquet cuff pressure conditioning

*Markers identified from fair quality papers.

3.5.4Conditioned pain modulation (CPM)

Pain during ascending (fingers first) and descending cold water immersion of the arm (elbows first) in HC and patients with FM was tested. One study demonstrated that HC felt less pain in their fingers during descending sessions compared to ascending, whereas patients with FM felt no difference [29]. Furthermore, it was demonstrated that patients with FM felt no changes in pain ratings after a pressure pain conditioning and a cold-water stimulation condition, whereas HC felt lower pain [50]. When comparing the efficacy of cold pressor test conditioning, one study [38] observed no CPM differences between both groups whereas another study [52] observed lower CPM efficacy in patients with FM compared to HC. When using tourniquet cuff conditioning, a study demonstrated that 95% of patients with FM showed inefficient CPM in comparison with zero HC cases [43]. However, lower PPT, HPT and higher pain ratings after a tourniquet cuff conditioning in patients with FM compared to HC were identified [51]. One study [53] observed that CPM decreases the nociceptive flexion reflex (NFR) amplitude in HC when painful conditioning was applied. However, in patients with FM, the nociceptive flexion reflex amplitudes were lower after applying non-painful conditioning CPM [53]. These findings are also shown in Table 5.

3.5.5Pain anticipation and catastrophizing

Some studies were conducted on pain anticipation and catastrophizing in patients with FM. It was demonstrated that patients showed lower responses in the ventral tegmental area, a dopamine-rich region, during pain anticipation compared to HC [49]. It was shown that patients who were more prone to catastrophizing had a lower pain threshold with cuff algometry [48]. Loggia et al. demonstrated that patients displaying lower pain anticipation, showed reduced activity in the lateral prefrontal cortex (LPC). By means of mediation analyses, it was shown that this reduced activity mediates the hyperalgesic effect of catastrophizing [48]. Oliva et al. showed no difference in attentional analgesia during concurrent thermal painful stimuli, calibrated to each individual’s pain threshold, between groups: both groups demonstrated a decrease in pain score during the hard task compared to the easy task [44]. One study demonstrated lower blood-pressure and cardiac stroke volume reactivity during a mental arithmetic task in patients with FM compared to the reactivity of ANS parameters during the cold pressor test [32].

Table 6 shows an overview of the identified markers, with an asterisk next to the markers identified from fair quality papers.

4.Discussion

In this review, patients with FM showed differences on HACS markers compared to healthy subjects. The markers identified to assess peripheral manifestation of HACS are higher pain after-sensation intensity (and lower decline rates), lower mechanical pain threshold detected by pin-prick stimulators,lower sound ‘pressure’ pain thresholds tested with auditory wideband noise testing, cutaneous silent period duration recorded with electrodes, abnormal autonomic nervous system responses to pain, higher slowly repeated evoked pain (SREP) sensitization (elicited by pressure stimuli) and lower nociceptive flexion reflex detected with electromyography. The markers identified to assess central manifestations of HACS are electroencephalogram (EEG) differences observed between FM and HC, brain and spinal activity variations (amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations (ALFF), region connectivity, neurologic pain signature response) detected with fMRI, brain perfusion differences observed on PET and SPECT scans, gray matter volume changes detected with voxel-based morphometry and cuff pressure conditioning.

4.1Measurements to assess peripheral manifestations of HACS

Peripheral assessments of HACS markers have provided inconsistent results. First of all, higher TSSP sensitivity in patients with FM compared to HC was shown in seven studies [19, 20, 23, 25, 26, 27, 28], with three of these studies being ranked fair quality [23, 27, 87], and four ranked poor quality [19, 20, 25, 26]. On the other hand, no TSSP difference between groups was found in two other studies [18, 21], with one study being ranked fair quality [21] and one with poor quality. From these findings, we cannot deduce that TSSP is a valid marker for the presence of HACS. However, the demonstrated higher pain after-sensation (AS) intensities [18, 26] and lower rates of pain AS decline [19, 20, 21] can support the suggestion to use them as markers for HACS in patients with FM. This is because HC showed opposite results and the higher pain sensation felt in patients with FM can be expressed through the higher pain AS intensities demonstrated in two studies [18, 26]. One study showed lower sound ‘pressure’ pain thresholds in patients with FM compared to HC, further expanding the noxious sensation spectrum of patients with FM to auditory mechanisms [42]. Regarding measurements of HPT and CPT (thermal sensory devices) [21, 34, 37, 39, 41] in patients with FM, one study [21] did not observe thermal pain threshold differences between patients with FM and HC. From these findings, and considering the fact that the study was only ranked fair quality, we cannot undoubtedly classify thermal pain thresholds as a usable marker for HACS identification in patients with FM. Lower MPT detected with pin-prick stimulators [34] in patients with FM compared to HC showed to be a promising tool for pain hypersensitivity detection in patients with FM. It is important to note that two of these studies [31, 41], were ranked fair quality. Furthermore, authors of several studies [23, 31, 34, 35, 36, 37, 38, 41, 42, 43, 44] demonstrated PPT measurements with pressure algometry to indicate the presence of hyperalgesia in patients with FM. However, one study found SREP specificity to be 25% higher (0.92) and sensitivity 4% higher (0.79) for discriminating between patients with FM and HC compared to PPT measurements (PPT 0.67) [31]. This may indicate that SREP, evoked by a series of pressure stimuli, is a better marker to discriminate for HACS between patients with FM and HC compared to PPT.The increased cutaneous silent period (CSP) duration after stimulation of the cutaneous nerve in patients with FM compared to HC represents a faster conduction of pain and longer period of sustained muscle contraction in patients [96]. This may suggest the effectivity of CSP as a peripheral marker for altered pain sensitization in FM.

By means of ECG, measuring ANS responses to cold pressor tests could be used in a clinical setting to detect abnormalities in the baroreflex responses in patients [29, 32]. The reduced baroreflex sensitivity and effectiveness during cold pressor test in patients with FM can be a manifestation of altered CNS activity. Additionally, the demonstrated reduced heart rate variability could be a result of a decreased baroreflex function in patients with FM [32]. Even though another study [33] showed no difference in heart rate increase during cold pressor test between both groups, results still indicated a higher heart rate and lower heart rate variability in patients with FM compared to HC at rest. Furthermore, the results on the correlation between pain modulation and motor features are inconsistent [36, 45, 46, 47], and we can therefore not deduce that assessing pain modulation during motor activity can be regarded currently as a valid marker.

4.2Measurements to assess central manifestations of HACS

Feasibility of measurements of central manifestations of HACS in patients with FM was shown in several studies [28, 43, 44, 48, 49, 53, 55, 56, 57, 58, 59, 60, 61, 62, 63, 64, 65, 66, 67, 68, 69, 70, 71]. However, except for three [49, 61, 87], these studies were qualified as poor quality. Furthermore, an explosive synchronization network is a network where a small perturbation rapidly propagates throughout the whole network. Explosive synchronization (ES) networks detected with EEG have shown to elicit higher sensitivity to external stimulation than non-ES networks [59]. Lee et al. [59] concluded that the presence of ES conditions in the brains of patients with FM can be an underlying mechanism of hypersensitivity. ES conditions may thus be a potential marker for HACS. Studies on LEP, on the contrary, yielded inconsistent results. Therefore, it cannot be concluded that LEP amplitude analysis with EEG is a valid method to assess HACS in patients with FM [72, 73, 74, 75].

Conditioned pain modulation (CPM) can be described as a painful conditioning stimulus leading to decreased pain intensity of another noxious stimulus [97]. The reduced NFR after non-painful conditioning (mechanical stimulation) in patients with FM points towards the presence of altered pain inhibitory pathways [53]. However, this was performed in a poor-quality study, which suggests that NFR amplitude measured with EMG after the application of a painful conditioning cannot be considered a valid marker for HACS detection. One additional study showed the positive effect of attentional analgesia in patients with FM, putting forward the capability of patients with FM to modulate pain when the given stimulus is sensitivity-adapted and the attentional task difficulty is correctly calibrated [44].

fMRI was used in eleven studies [28, 44, 48, 49, 61, 62, 63, 64, 65, 66, 67] to examine various aspects of HACS in patients with FM. The variations of mean amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations (ALFF) in patients with FM indicate an imbalance between pain and sensory processes [65]. This suggests the presence of altered central nervous system activity in patients with FM [65]. fMRI showed increased connectivity between various brain regions in patients with FM compared to HC [63, 64, 66], reflecting the expression of HACS in pain processing mechanisms in patients with FM [63]. Furthermore, increased brain activation of pain-related areas during non-painful stimulation in patients with FM indicate physiological evidence of their increased pain perception [62]. Similar patterns of brain activation after sensitivity-adjusted painful stimuli in both groups also suggest increased pain sensitivity in patients with FM [28, 44]. The same study found a positive correlation between spinal activation during TSSP and increased BOLD activity in the brainstem, suggesting a different pain modulation mechanism in patients with FM [28]. The higher neurologic pain signature (NPS) responses, an fMRI-based neurologic correlate of physical pain, provides evidence of amplified pain processing and HACS in patients with FM [61]. These studies help us conclude that fMRI is a useful tool to help indicate the following markers of HACS in patients with FM: amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations (ALFF) variations, brain activity and connectivity differences, neurologic pain signature responses and pain anticipation dysfunction. On the other hand, rCBF variations indicate patient’ higher attention to innocuous sensory signals at rest. These findings make PET and SPECT imaging potential tools for the investigation of brain perfusion abnormalities [68, 69, 70]. Lastly, two studies [43, 51] point out the potential role of CPM assessment with tourniquet pain conditioning as a marker for HACS in patients with FM [51].

All taken together, seventy-four studies were ranked as poor or fair quality. Those studies indicate a high risk of bias, which should be taken into consideration when interpreting results. The markers identified from studies ranked as poor quality cannot be determined as being as valid as markers identified in higher quality papers. Out of the markers identified in this review, the following were suggested from at least fair quality papers: higher SREP sensitization (elicited by pressure stimuli) [30, 31], NPS response detected with fMRI [61], lower sound ‘pressure’ pain thresholds [42], brain perfusion differences [81, 87] and conditioned pain modulation with cuff pressure conditioning [51]. The lower pain AS decline rates were suggested from three papers [19, 20, 21], out of which only one is fair quality [21].

A limitation to this review is the fact that due to the heterogeneity of the studies, especially in the vast number of markers, measurements and differences in study protocols, a meta-analysis could not be conducted. The current study has implications in the clinical setting, because these findings can be utilized to construct a more objective diagnostic protocol for HACS assessment in patients with FM. Furthermore, assessing HACS development over time as a proxy for disease progression in the day-to-day clinical practice may be valuable. Questionnaires combined with a short battery of objective tests, grouping the markers and their respective tools could be a solution to objectively quantify patients pain markers. Markers that are best executable and affordable in daily practice are tourniquet cuff pressure conditioning [51] and pressure stimuli, the latter being derived from a fair quality paper [31]. By assessing these markers, HACS may be more objectively quantified Additionally, the diagnosis of HACS development over time can also be combined with methods which do not require questionnaires or markers [10]. Physicians could strengthen the diagnosis by assessing amplified pain distribution (number of pain regions and/or pain intensity per region) compared to previous assessments. This will ultimately help to make a personalized treatment plan for daily clinical practice. Studies have shown that the implementation of physical and pharmacological therapy in patients with temporomandibular disorders has led to the reduction of pain- and mobility-related symptoms [6]. Hence, FM, as an overlapping chronic pain disorder with relations to central nervous system dysfunction due to HACS, could also benefit from physical therapy for the rehabilitation of HACS and, as result, for the improvement of pain. Further research, however, is warranted to validate these hypotheses. It is important to note that there is currently no single test or gold standard that can identify patients with HACS and that a combination of different measurements could formulate a gold standard, possibly also combined with more invasive markers which were left outside the scope of the current study.

5.Conclusion

The current study identified non-invasive markers for peripheral manifestations of HACS in FM including quantitative sensory testing measurements and nociceptive flexion reflex assessment. This study also revealed that various techniques can be used to assess the aforementioned HACS. Among them are markers such as EMG for the assessment of nociceptive flexion reflex. Lastly, conditioned pain modulation by tourniquet cuff pain conditioning and techniques such as EEG, PET, SPECT, fMRI and VBM were also identified to be useful in the assessment of central manifestations of HACS. More studies should be conducted in order to determine which markers can clinically be used to identify HACS in patients with FM.

Funding

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Competing interests

None to declare.

Supplementary data

The supplementary files are available to download from http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/BMR-220430.

References

[1] 

IASP. Pain Terminology. Nociplastic pain. https://www.iasp-pain.org/resources/terminology/ (assessed at December 5, (2023) ).

[2] 

Chimenti RL, Frey-Law LA, Sluka KA. A Mechanism-Based Approach to Physical Therapist Management of Pain. Phys Ther. (2018) ; 98: (5): 302-14.

[3] 

Nijs J, George SZ, Clauw DJ, Fernández-de-las-Peñas C, Kosek E, Ickmans K, et al. Central sensitisation in chronic pain conditions: latest discoveries and their potential for precision medicine. The Lancet Rheumatology. (2021) ; 3: (5): e383-e92.

[4] 

Woolf CJ. Central sensitization: implications for the diagnosis and treatment of pain. Pain. (2011) ; 152: (3 Suppl): S2-S15.

[5] 

Staud R. Evidence for shared pain mechanisms in osteoarthritis, low back pain, and fibromyalgia. Curr Rheumatol Rep. (2011) ; 13: (6): 513-20.

[6] 

Ferrillo M, Giudice A, Marotta N, Fortunato F, Di Venere D, Ammendolia A, et al. Pain Management and Rehabilitation for Central Sensitization in Temporomandibular Disorders: A Comprehensive Review. Int J Mol Sci. (2022) ; 23: (20).

[7] 

Paolucci T, de Sire A, Ferrillo M, di Fabio D, Molluso A, Patruno A, et al. Telerehabilitation proposal of mind-body technique for physical and psychological outcomes in patients with fibromyalgia. Front Physiol. (2022) ; 13: : 917956.

[8] 

Vrouva S, Sopidou V, Koutsioumpa E, Chanopoulos K, Nikolopoulou A, Papatsimpas V, et al. Can Exercise Affect the Pain Characteristics in Patients with Fibromyalgia? A Randomized Controlled Trial. Healthcare. (2022) ; 10: (12): 2426.

[9] 

Schuttert I, Timmerman H, Petersen KK, McPhee ME, Arendt-Nielsen L, Reneman MF, et al. The Definition, Assessment, and Prevalence of (Human Assumed) Central Sensitisation in Patients with Chronic Low Back Pain: A Systematic Review. J Clin Med. (2021) ; 10: (24).

[10] 

Giacomelli C, Sernissi F, Rossi A, Bombardieri S, Bazzichi L. Biomarkers in fibromyalgia: a review. Current Biomarker Findings. (2014) .

[11] 

Malatji BG, Meyer H, Mason S, Engelke UFH, Wevers RA, van Reenen M, et al. A diagnostic biomarker profile for fibromyalgia syndrome based on an NMR metabolomics study of selected patients and controls. BMC Neurol. (2017) ; 17: (1): 88.

[12] 

Russell IJ, Michalek JE, Vipraio GA, Fletcher EM, Javors MA, Bowden CA. Platelet 3H-imipramine uptake receptor density and serum serotonin levels in patients with fibromyalgia/fibrositis syndrome. J Rheumatol. (1992) ; 19: (1): 104-9.

[13] 

Russell IJ, Orr MD, Littman B, Vipraio GA, Alboukrek D, Michalek JE, et al. Elevated cerebrospinal fluid levels of substance P in patients with the fibromyalgia syndrome. Arthritis Rheum. (1994) ; 37: (11): 1593-601.

[14] 

Bhargava J, Hurley JA. Fibromyalgia. Stat Pearls. Treasure Island (FL) (2020) .

[15] 

Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, Group P. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA Statement. Open Med. (2009) ; 3: (3): e123-30.

[16] 

National heart labi. Study Quality Assessment Tools [Available from: https://wwwnhlbi.nih.gov/health-topics/study-quality-assessment-tools].

[17] 

Paudel S, Owen AJ, Owusu-Addo E, Smith BJ. Physical activity participation and the risk of chronic diseases among South Asian adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sci Rep. (2019) ; 9: (1): 9771.

[18] 

Bosma RL, Mojarad EA, Leung L, Pukall C, Staud R, Stroman PW. FMRI of spinal and supra-spinal correlates of temporal pain summation in fibromyalgia patients, 1349-60. doi: 10.002/hbm.23106.

[19] 

Staud R, Robinson ME, Price DD. Temporal Summation of Second Pain and Its Maintenance Are Useful for Characterizing Widespread Central Sensitization of Fibromyalgia Patients. Journal of Pain. (2007) ; 8: (11): 893-901.

[20] 

Staud R, Price Dd Fau-Robinson ME, Robinson Me Fau-Mauderli AP, Mauderli Ap Fau-Vierck CJ, Vierck CJ. Maintenance of windup of second pain requires less frequent stimulation in fibromyalgia patients compared to normal controls, 689-96.

[21] 

Janal MN, Raphael KG, Cook DB, Sirois DA, Nemelivsky L, Staud R. Thermal temporal summation and decay of after-sensations in temporomandibular myofascial pain patients with and without comorbid fibromyalgia, 641-52. doi: 10.2147/JPR.S109038.

[22] 

Staud R, Weyl EE, Riley IJL, Fillingim RB. Slow temporal summation of pain for assessment of central pain sensitivity and clinical pain of fibromyalgia patients. PLoS ONE. (2014) ; 9: (2).

[23] 

Goubert D, Danneels L, Graven-Nielsen T, Descheemaeker F, Meeus M. Differences in Pain Processing Between Patients with Chronic Low Back Pain, Recurrent Low Back Pain, and Fibromyalgia. 307-18.

[24] 

Staud R, Bovee CE, Robinson ME, Price DD. Cutaneous C-fiber pain abnormalities of fibromyalgia patients are specifically related to temporal summation. Pain. (2008) ; 139: (2): 315-23.

[25] 

Price DD, Staud R, Fau-Robinson ME, Robinson Me Fau-Mauderli AP, Mauderli Ap Fau-Cannon R, Cannon R, Fau-Vierck CJ, Vierck CJ. Enhanced temporal summation of second pain and its central modulation in fibromyalgia patients. 49-59.

[26] 

Staud R, Cannon Rc Fau-Mauderli AP, Mauderli Ap Fau-Robinson ME, Robinson Me Fau-Price DD, Price Dd Fau-Vierck CJ, Jr., Vierck CJ, Jr. Temporal summation of pain from mechanical stimulation of muscle tissue in normal controls and subjects with fibromyalgia syndrome. 87-95.

[27] 

Staud R, Robinson ME, Vierck CJ, Jr., Price DD. Diffuse noxious inhibitory controls (DNIC) attenuate temporal summation of second pain in normal males but not in normal females or fibromyalgia patients. Pain. (2003) ; 101: (1-2): 167-74.

[28] 

Staud R, Boissoneault J, Lai S, Mejia MS, Ramanlal R, Godfrey MM, et al. Spinal cord neural activity of patients with fibromyalgia and healthy controls during temporal summation of pain: an fMRI study. J Neurophysiol. (2021) ; 126: (3): 946-56.

[29] 

Chalaye P, Goffaux P, Bourgault P, Lafrenaye S, Devroede G, Watier A, et al. Comparing pain modulation and autonomic responses in fibromyalgia and irritable bowel syndrome patients. Clinical Journal of Pain. (2012) ; 28: (6): 519-26.

[30] 

de la Coba P, Bruehl S, Duschek S, Reyes Del Paso GA. Blood pressure-related pain modulation in fibromyalgia: Differentiating between static versus dynamic pain indicators 79-85. LID-S0167-8760(18)30075-8; doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2018.10.006.

[31] 

de la Coba P, Bruehl S, Moreno-Padilla M, del Paso GAR. Responses to slowly repeated evoked pain stimuli in fibromyalgia patients: Evidence of enhanced pain sensitization. Pain Medicine (United States). (2017) ; 18: (9): 1778-86.

[32] 

del Paso GAR, Contreras-Merino AM, Coba P, Duschek S. The cardiac, vasomotor, and myocardial branches of the baroreflex in fibromyalgia: Associations with pain, affective impairments, sleep problems, and fatigue. Psychophysiology. (2021) ; 58: (5).

[33] 

Hazra S, Venkataraman S, Handa G, Yadav SL, Wadhwa S, Singh U, et al. A cross-sectional study on central sensitization and autonomic changes in fibromyalgia. Frontiers in Neuroscience. (2020) ; 14: .

[34] 

Blumenstiel K, Gerhardt A, Fau-Rolke R, Rolke R, Fau-Bieber C, Bieber C, Fau-Tesarz J, Tesarz J, Fau-Friederich H-C, Friederich HC, Fau-Eich W, et al. Quantitative sensory testing profiles in chronic back pain are distinct from those in fibromyalgia, 682-90. doi: 10.1097/AJP.0b013e3182177654.

[35] 

Jespersen A, Dreyer L, Kendall S, Graven-Nielsen T, Arendt-Nielsen L, Bliddal H, et al. Computerized cuff pressure algometry: A new method to assess deep-tissue hypersensitivity in fibromyalgia. Pain. (2007) ; 131: (1-2): 57-62.

[36] 

Kosek E, Ekholm J, Hansson P. Increased pressure pain sensibility in fibromyalgia patients is located deep to the skin but not restricted to muscle tissue. Pain. (1995) ; 63: (3): 335-9.

[37] 

Kosek E, Ekholm J, Hansson P. Sensory dysfunction in fibromyalgia patients with implications for pathogenic mechanisms. Pain. (1996) ; 68: (2-3): 375-83.

[38] 

van Vliet J, Tieleman AA, Verrips A, Timmerman H, van Dongen RTM, van Engelen BGM, et al. Qualitative and Quantitative Aspects of Pain in Patients With Myotonic Dystrophy Type 2.920-30. LID-S1526-5900(18)30117-2; doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2018.03.006.

[39] 

Hurtig IM, Raak Ri Fau-Kendall SA, Kendall Sa Fau-Gerdle B, Gerdle B, Fau-Wahren LK, Wahren LK. Quantitative sensory testing in fibromyalgia patients and in healthy subjects: identification of subgroups. 316-22.

[40] 

Staud R, Weyl Ee Fau-Price DD, Price Dd Fau-Robinson ME, Robinson ME. Mechanical and heat hyperalgesia highly predict clinical pain intensity in patients with chronic musculoskeletal pain syndromes. 725-35; doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2012..

[41] 

Gerhardt A, Eich W, Janke S, Leisner S, Treede RD, Tesarz J. Chronic Widespread Back Pain is Distinct From Chronic Local Back Pain: Evidence From Quantitative Sensory Testing, Pain Drawings, and Psychometrics. Clin J Pain. (2016) ; 32: (7): 568-79.

[42] 

Staud R, Godfrey MM, Robinson ME. Fibromyalgia Patients Are Not Only Hypersensitive to Painful Stimuli But Also to Acoustic Stimuli. J Pain. (2021) ; 22: (8): 914-25.

[43] 

Bourke JH, Wodehouse T, Clark LV, Constantinou E, Kidd BL, Langford R, et al. Central sensitisation in chronic fatigue syndrome and fibromyalgia; a case control study. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. (2021) ; 150: .

[44] 

Oliva V, Gregory R, Brooks JCW, Pickering AE. Central pain modulatory mechanisms of attentional analgesia are preserved in fibromyalgia. Pain. (2022) ; 163: (1): 125-36.

[45] 

Staud R, Robinson Me Fau-Weyl EE, Weyl Ee Fau-Price DD, Price DD. Pain variability in fibromyalgia is related to activity and rest: role of peripheral tissue impulse input, 1376-83. doi: 10.016/j.jpain.2010.03.011.

[46] 

Staud R, Robinson ME, Price DD. Isometric exercise has opposite effects on central pain mechanisms in fibromyalgia patients compared to normal controls. Pain. (2005) ; 118: (1-2): 176-84.

[47] 

McLoughlin MJ, Stegner AJ, Cook DB. The relationship between physical activity and brain responses to pain in fibromyalgia. Journal of Pain. (2011) ; 12: (6): 640-51.

[48] 

Loggia ML, Berna C, Kim J, Cahalan CM, Martel MO, Gollub RL, et al. The lateral prefrontal cortex mediates the hyperalgesic effects of negative cognitions in chronic pain patients, 692-9; doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2015.04.003 LID-S1526-5900(15)00639-2 [pii].

[49] 

Loggia ML, Berna C, Fau-Kim J, Kim J, Fau-Cahalan CM, Cahalan Cm Fau-Gollub RL, Gollub Rl Fau-Wasan AD, Wasan Ad Fau-Harris RE, et al. Disrupted brain circuitry for pain-related reward/punishment in fibromyalgia, 203-12. doi: 10.1002/art.38191.

[50] 

Schoen CJ, Ablin JN, Ichesco E, Bhavsar RJ, Kochlefl L, Harris RE, et al. A novel paradigm to evaluate conditioned pain modulation in fibromyalgia, 711-9.

[51] 

Vaegter HB, Graven-Nielsen T. Pain modulatory phenotypes differentiate subgroups with different clinical and experimental pain sensitivity, 1480-8. doi: 10.097/j.pain.0000000000000543.

[52] 

Potvin S, Larouche A, Fau-Normand E, Normand E, Fau-de Souza JB, de Souza Jb Fau-Gaumond I, Gaumond I, Fau-Grignon S, Grignon S, Fau-Marchand S, et al. DRD3 Ser9Gly polymorphism is related to thermal pain perception and modulation in chronic widespread pain patients and healthy controls, 969-75. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2009.03.013.

[53] 

Desmeules JA, Cedraschi C, Fau-Rapiti E, Rapiti E, Fau-Baumgartner E, Baumgartner E, Fau-Finckh A, Finckh A, Fau-Cohen P, Cohen P, Fau-Dayer P, et al. Neurophysiologic evidence for a central sensitization in patients with fibromyalgia, 1420-9.

[54] 

Morris V, Cruwys S, Fau-Kidd B, Kidd B. Increased capsaicin-induced secondary hyperalgesia as a marker of abnormal sensory activity in patients with fibromyalgia, 205-7.

[55] 

Martinsen S, Flodin P, Berrebi J, Lofgren M, Bileviciute-Ljungar I, Ingvar M, et al. Fibromyalgia patients had normal distraction related pain inhibition but cognitive impairment reflected in caudate nucleus and hippocampus during the Stroop Color Word Teste, 108637. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0108637.

[56] 

Montoya P, Pauli P, Batra A, Wiedemann G. Altered processing of pain-related information in patients with fibromyalgia. European Journal of Pain. (2005) ; 9: (3): 293.

[57] 

Banic B, Petersen-Felix S, Andersen OK, Radanov BP, Villiger PM, Arendt-Nielsen L, et al. Evidence for spinal cord hypersensitivity in chronic pain after whiplash injury and in fibromyalgia. Pain. (2004) ; 107: (1-2): 7-15.

[58] 

Desmeules J, Chabert J, Rebsamen M, Rapiti E, Piguet V, Besson M, et al. Central pain sensitization, COMT Val158Met polymorphism, and emotional factors in fibromyalgia, 129-35. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2013.10.004; LID-S1526-5900(13) 01299-6 [pii].

[59] 

Lee U, Kim M, Lee K, Kaplan CM, Clauw DJ, Kim S, et al. Functional Brain Network Mechanism of Hypersensitivity in Chronic Pain. Scientific Reports. (2018) ; 8: (1): 243.

[60] 

Fallon N, Chiu YH, Li X, Nurmikko TJ, Stancak A. Ipsilateral cortical activation in fibromyalgia patients during brushing correlates with symptom severity. Clinical Neurophysiology. (2013) ; 124: (1): 154-63.

[61] 

López-Solà M, Woo CW, Pujol J, Deus J, Harrison BJ, Monfort J, et al. Towards a neurophysiological signature for fibromyalgia. Pain. (2017) ; 158: (1): 34-47.

[62] 

Cook DB, Lange G, Ciccone DS, Liu WC, Steffener J, Natelson BH. Functional Imaging of Pain in Patients with Primary Fibromyalgia. Journal of Rheumatology. (2004) ; 31: (2): 364-78.

[63] 

Craggs JG, Staud R, Fau-Robinson ME, Robinson Me Fau-Perlstein WM, Perlstein Wm Fau-Price DD, Price DD. Effective connectivity among brain regions associated with slow temporal summation of C-fiber-evoked pain in fibromyalgia patients and healthy controls, 390-400. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2012.01.002.

[64] 

Ichesco E, Puiu T, Hampson JP, Kairys AE, Clauw DJ, Harte SE, et al. Altered fMRI resting-state connectivity in individuals with fibromyalgia on acute pain stimulation, 1079-89. doi: 10.02/ej832.

[65] 

Martucci KT, Weber KA, Mackey SC. Altered Cervical Spinal Cord Resting-State Activity in Fibromyalgia. Arthritis and Rheumatology. (2019) ; 71: (3): 441-50.

[66] 

Staud R, Craggs Jg Fau-Perlstein WM, Perlstein Wm Fau-Robinson ME, Robinson Me Fau-Price DD, Price DD. Brain activity associated with slow temporal summation of C-fiber evoked pain in fibromyalgia patients and healthy controls, 1078-89. doi: 10.16/j.ejpain.2008.02.002.

[67] 

Giesecke T, Gracely RH, Grant MA, Nachemson A, Petzke F, Williams DA, et al. Evidence of augmented central pain processing in idiopathic chronic low back pain. Arthritis Rheum. (2004) ; 50: (2): 613-23.

[68] 

Wik G, Fischer H, Fau-Bragee B, Bragee B Fau-Kristianson M, Kristianson M, Fau-Fredrikson M, Fredrikson M. Retrosplenial cortical activation in the fibromyalgia syndrome, 619-21.

[69] 

Wik G, Fischer H, Fau-Finer B, Finer B, Fau-Bragee B, Bragee B, Fau-Kristianson M, Kristianson M, Fau-Fredrikson M, Fredrikson M. Retrospenial cortical deactivation during painful stimulation of fibromyalgic patients, 1-8.

[70] 

Guedj E, Taieb D, Fau-Cammilleri S, Cammilleri S, Fau-Lussato D, Lussato D, Fau-de Laforte C, de Laforte C, Fau-Niboyet J, Niboyet J, Fau-Mundler O, et al. 99mTc-ECD brain perfusion SPECT in hyperalgesic fibromyalgia, 130-4.

[71] 

Burgmer M, Gaubitz M, Fau-Konrad C, Konrad C, Fau-Wrenger M, Wrenger M Fau-Hilgart S, Hilgart S, Fau-Heuft G, Heuft G, Fau-Pfleiderer B, et al. Decreased gray matter volumes in the cingulo-frontal cortex and the amygdala in patients with fibromyalgia, 566-73. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181a32da0.

[72] 

Lorenz J. Hyperalgesia or hypervigilance? An evoked potential approach to the study of fibromyalgia syndrome.19-22.

[73] 

De Tommaso M, Nolano M, Iannone F, Vecchio E, Ricci K, Lorenzo M, et al. Update on laser-evoked potential findings in fibromyalgia patients in light of clinical and skin biopsy features. Journal of Neurology. (2014) ; 261: (3): 461-72.

[74] 

Van Assche DCF, Plaghki L, Masquelier E, Hatem SM. Fibromyalgia syndrome – A laser-evoked potentials study unsupportive of small nerve fibre involvement. European Journal of Pain (United Kingdom). (2020) ; 24: (2): 448-56.

[75] 

Vecchio E, Lombardi R, Paolini M, Libro G, Delussi M, Ricci K, et al. Peripheral and central nervous system correlates in fibromyalgia. European Journal of pain (London, England). (2020) .

[76] 

Maestu C, Cortes A, Vazquez JM, del Rio D, Gomez-Arguelles JM, del Pozo F, et al. Increased brain responses during subjectively-matched mechanical pain stimulation in fibromyalgia patients as evidenced by MEG. Clinical Neurophysiology. (2013) ; 124: (4): 752-60.

[77] 

Lõpez-Solà M, Pujol J, Wager TD, Garcia-Fontanals A, Blanco-Hinojo L, Garcia-Blanco S, et al. Altered functional magnetic resonance imaging responses to nonpainful sensory stimulation in fibromyalgia patients. Arthritis and Rheumatology. (2014) ; 66: (11): 3200-9.

[78] 

Pujol J, López-Solà M, Ortiz H, Vilanova JC, Harrison BJ, Yücel M, et al. Mapping brain response to pain in fibromyalgia patients using temporal analysis of fMRI. PLoS ONE. (2009) ; 4: (4).

[79] 

Burgmer M, Pfleiderer B, Fau-Maihofner C, Maihofner C, Fau-Gaubitz M, Gaubitz M, Fau-Wessolleck E, Wessolleck E, Fau-Heuft G, Heuft G, Fau-Pogatzki-Zahn E, et al. Cerebral mechanisms of experimental hyperalgesia in fibromyalgia, 636-47. doi: 10.1002/j.532-2149.011.00058.x.

[80] 

Ichesco E, Schmidt-Wilcke T, Bhavsar R, Clauw DJ, Peltier SJ, Kim J, et al. Altered resting state connectivity of the insular cortex in individuals with fibromyalgia, 815-26.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2014.04.007; LID-S1526-5900(14)00700-7 [pii].

[81] 

Donadel DG, Zortea M, Torres ILS, Fregni F, Caumo W. The mapping of cortical activation by near-infrared spectroscopy might be a biomarker related to the severity of fibromyalgia symptoms. Scientific Reports. (2021) ; 11: (1): 15754.

[82] 

Maestú C, Blanco M, Nevado A, Romero J, Rodríguez-Rubio P, Galindo J, et al. Reduction of pain thresholds in fibromyalgia after very low-intensity magnetic stimulation: A double-blinded, randomized placebo-controlled clinical trial. Pain Research and Management. (2013) ; 18: (6): e101-e6.

[83] 

Matthey A, Cedraschi C, Fau-Piguet V, Piguet V, Fau-Besson M, Besson M, Fau-Chabert J, Chabert J, Fau-Daali Y, Daali Y, Fau-Courvoisier D, et al. Dual reuptake inhibitor milnacipran and spinal pain pathways in fibromyalgia patients: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trialE, 553-62.

[84] 

Passard A, Attal N, Fau-Benadhira R, Benadhira R, Fau-Brasseur L, Brasseur L, Fau-Saba G, Saba G, Fau-Sichere P, Sichere P, Fau-Perrot S, et al. Effects of unilateral repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the motor cortex on chronic widespread pain in fibromyalgia, 2661-70.

[85] 

Staud R, Lucas YE, Price DD, Robinson ME. Effects of milnacipran on clinical pain and hyperalgesia of patients with fibromyalgia: results of a 6-week randomized controlled trial, 750-9. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2015.04.010; LID-S1526-5900 (15)00664-1 [pii].

[86] 

Zhang Y, Ahmed S, Vo T, St Hilaire K, Houghton M, Cohen AS, et al. Increased pain sensitivity in chronic pain subjects on opioid therapy: a cross-sectional study using quantitative sensory testing, 911-22. doi: 10.1111/pme.12606; [doi].

[87] 

Staud R, Boissoneault J, Lai S, Mejia MS, Ramanlal R, Godfrey MM, et al. Spinal cord neural activity of patients with fibromyalgia and healthy controls during temporal summation of pain: An fMRI study. Journal of Neurophysiology. (2021) ; 126: (3): 946-56.

[88] 

Wodehouse TA-Ohoo, Poply K, Ramaswamy S, Snidvongs S, Bourke J, Tahir H, et al. A pilot study investigating whether quantitative sensory testing alters after treatment in patients with fibromyalgia, 250-6. doi: 10.1177/2049463718776336.

[89] 

Baek SH, Seok HY, Koo YS, Kim BJ. Lengthened cutaneous silent period in fibromyalgia suggesting central sensitization as a pathogenesis. PLoS ONE. (2016) ; 11: (2).

[90] 

Bendtsen L, NØrregaard J, Jensen R, Olesen J. Evidence of qualitatively altered nociception in patients with fibromyalgia. Arthritis and Rheumatism. (1997) ; 40: (1): 98-102.

[91] 

Fallon N, Alghamdi J, Fau-Chiu Y, Chiu Y, Fau-Sluming V, Sluming V, Fau-Nurmikko T, Nurmikko T, Fau-Stancak A, Stancak A. Structural alterations in brainstem of fibromyalgia syndrome patients correlate with sensitivity to mechanical pressure, 163-70. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.07.011.

[92] 

Gentile E, Brunetti A, Ricci K, Delussi M, Bevilacqua V, de Tommaso M. Mutual interaction between motor cortex activation and pain in fibromyalgia: EEG-fNIRS study. PLoS ONE. (2020) ; 15: (1).

[93] 

Gerdle B, Gronlund C, Fau-Karlsson SJ, Karlsson Sj Fau-Holtermann A, Holtermann A, Fau-Roeleveld K, Roeleveld K. Altered neuromuscular control mechanisms of the trapezius muscle in fibromyalgia, 42. doi: 10.1186/471-2474-11-42.

[94] 

Lim M, Roosink M, Kim JS, Kim DJ, Kim HW, Lee EB, et al. Disinhibition of the primary somatosensory cortex in patients with fibromyalgia. Pain. (2015) ; 156: (4): 666-74.

[95] 

Truini A, Gerardi MC, Di Stefano G, La Cesa S, Iannuccelli C, Pepe A, et al. Hyperexcitability in pain matrices in patients with fibromyalgia, S68-72.

[96] 

Al-Mahdawi A, Sami S, Hamdan F. Electrodiagnostic study in patients with fibromyalgia: Implication for central sensitization. Indian Journal of Rheumatology. (2021) ; 16: (3): 263-8.

[97] 

Damien J, Colloca L, Bellei-Rodriguez CE, Marchand S. Pain Modulation: From Conditioned Pain Modulation to Placebo and Nocebo Effects in Experimental and Clinical Pain. Int Rev Neurobiol. (2018) ; 139: : 255-96.

[98] 

Staud R, Godfrey MM, Robinson ME. Fibromyalgia Patients Are Not Only Hypersensitive to Painful Stimuli But Also to Acoustic Stimuli. Journal of Pain. (2021) ; 22: (8): 914-25.