Searching for just a few words should be enough to get started. If you need to make more complex queries, use the tips below to guide you.
Article type: Review Article
Authors: Mendez, Mario F.a; b; c; *
Affiliations: [a] V.A. Greater Los Angeles Healthcare System, Los Angeles, CA, USA | [b] Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry & Biobehavioral Sciences, Los Angeles, CA, USA | [c] David Geffen School of Medicine, University of California at Los Angeles, CA, USA
Correspondence: [*] Correspondence to: Mario F. Mendez, MD, PhD, Neurobehavior Service (116AF), West Los Angeles VA Healthcare Center; 11301 Wilshire Blvd, Los Angeles, CA 90073, USA. Tel.: +1 310 478 3711, extension 41581; Fax: 310 268 4181; E-mail: mmendez@ucla.edu.
Abstract: Despite the large number of elderly bilinguals at risk for Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and dementia worldwide, significant questions remain about the relationship between speaking more than one language and later cognitive decline. Bilingualism may impact on cognitive and neural reserve, time of onset of dementia symptoms and neuropathology, and linguistic competency in dementia. This review indicates increased cognitive reserve from executive (monitoring, selecting, inhibiting) control of two languages and increased neural reserve involving left frontal and related areas for language control. Many, but not all, studies indicate a delay in dementia symptom onset but worse hippocampal and mesiotemporal atrophy among bilinguals versus monolinguals with AD. In contrast, bilinguals do worse on language measures, and bilinguals with AD or dementia have difficulty maintaining and monitoring their second language. Together, these studies suggest that early-acquired and proficient bilingualism increases reserve through frontal-predominant executive control, and these executive abilities compensate for early dementia symptoms, delaying their onset but not the neuropathology of their disease. Finally, as executive control decreases further with advancing dementia, there is increasing difficulty inhibiting the dominant first language and staying in the second language. These conclusions must be interpreted with caution, given the problems inherent in this type of research; however, they do recommend more work on the pre-dementia neuroprotective effects and the dementia-related language impairments of bilingualism.
Keywords: Alzheimer’s disease, bilingualism, dementia, language
DOI: 10.3233/JAD-190397
Journal: Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, vol. 71, no. 2, pp. 377-388, 2019
IOS Press, Inc.
6751 Tepper Drive
Clifton, VA 20124
USA
Tel: +1 703 830 6300
Fax: +1 703 830 2300
sales@iospress.com
For editorial issues, like the status of your submitted paper or proposals, write to editorial@iospress.nl
IOS Press
Nieuwe Hemweg 6B
1013 BG Amsterdam
The Netherlands
Tel: +31 20 688 3355
Fax: +31 20 687 0091
info@iospress.nl
For editorial issues, permissions, book requests, submissions and proceedings, contact the Amsterdam office info@iospress.nl
Inspirees International (China Office)
Ciyunsi Beili 207(CapitaLand), Bld 1, 7-901
100025, Beijing
China
Free service line: 400 661 8717
Fax: +86 10 8446 7947
china@iospress.cn
For editorial issues, like the status of your submitted paper or proposals, write to editorial@iospress.nl
如果您在出版方面需要帮助或有任何建, 件至: editorial@iospress.nl