Searching for just a few words should be enough to get started. If you need to make more complex queries, use the tips below to guide you.
Article type: Research Article
Authors: Simon, Sharon S.a | Tusch, Erich S.a | Feng, Nicole C.a | Håkansson, Kristerb; c | Mohammed, Abdul H.c; d | Daffner, Kirk R.a; *
Affiliations: [a] Department of Neurology, Center for Brain/Mind Medicine, Division of Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA | [b] Division of Clinical Geriatrics, Department of NVS, Karolinska Insitutet, Stockholm, Sweden | [c] Department of Psychology, Linnaeus University, Växjö, Sweden | Center of Alzheimer Research, Department of Neurobiology, Karolinska Insitutet, Stockholm, Sweden
Correspondence: [*] Correspondence to: Kirk R. Daffner, MD, FAAN, Center for Brain/Mind Medicine, Division of Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, 60 Fenwood Road, Boston, MA 02115, USA. Tel.: +1 617 732 8060; Fax: +1 617 738 9122; E-mail: kdaffner@bwh.harvard.edu.
Abstract: Background:Developing effective interventions to attenuate age-related cognitive decline and prevent or delay the onset of dementia are major public health goals. Computerized cognitive training (CCT) has been marketed increasingly to older adults, but its efficacy remains unclear. Working memory (WM), a key determinant of higher order cognitive abilities, is susceptible to age-related decline and a relevant target for CCT in elders. Objective:To evaluate the efficacy of CCT focused on WM compared to an active control condition in healthy older adults. Methods:Eighty-two cognitively normal adults from two sites (USA and Sweden) were randomly assigned to Cogmed Adaptive or Non-Adaptive (active control) CCT groups. Training was performed in participants’ homes, five days per week over five weeks. Changes in the performance of the Cogmed trained tasks, and in five neuropsychological tests (Trail Making Test Part A and Part B, Digit Symbol, Controlled Oral Word Association Test and Semantic Fluency) were used as outcome measures. Results:The groups were comparable at baseline. The Adaptive group showed robust gains in the trained tasks, and there was a time-by-group interaction for the Digit Symbol test, with significant improvement only after Adaptive training. In addition, the magnitude of the intervention effect was similar at both sites. Conclusion:Home-based CCT Adaptive WM training appears more effective than Non-Adaptive training in older adults from different cultural backgrounds. We present evidence of improvement in trained tasks and on a demanding untrained task dependent upon WM and processing speed. The benefits over the active control group suggest that the Adaptive CCT gains were linked to providing a continuously challenging level of WM difficulty.
Keywords: Aging, computerized cognitive training, randomized controlled trial, working memory
DOI: 10.3233/JAD-180455
Journal: Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, vol. 65, no. 3, pp. 931-949, 2018
IOS Press, Inc.
6751 Tepper Drive
Clifton, VA 20124
USA
Tel: +1 703 830 6300
Fax: +1 703 830 2300
sales@iospress.com
For editorial issues, like the status of your submitted paper or proposals, write to editorial@iospress.nl
IOS Press
Nieuwe Hemweg 6B
1013 BG Amsterdam
The Netherlands
Tel: +31 20 688 3355
Fax: +31 20 687 0091
info@iospress.nl
For editorial issues, permissions, book requests, submissions and proceedings, contact the Amsterdam office info@iospress.nl
Inspirees International (China Office)
Ciyunsi Beili 207(CapitaLand), Bld 1, 7-901
100025, Beijing
China
Free service line: 400 661 8717
Fax: +86 10 8446 7947
china@iospress.cn
For editorial issues, like the status of your submitted paper or proposals, write to editorial@iospress.nl
如果您在出版方面需要帮助或有任何建, 件至: editorial@iospress.nl