Honor Endorsement in Male Youth: A General Risk Factor for Aggressive Behavior?
Abstract
Honor refers to socially conferred self-worth, strong norms of reciprocity, and direct retaliation for transgressions. Honor norms have been discussed as relevant in explaining aggressive behavior, particularly for immigrant groups. In this study we examined the endorsement of honor norms and their association with aggressive behavior in a culturally diverse sample in Germany. Online questionnaire data were used from male adolescents aged 11–18 years (N = 774; Mage = 15.37, 34.2% with a migration background). Multiple linear regression analyses indicated that students of Turkish and Russian origin endorsed honor more than students without a migration background, whereas students of Polish origin did not differ. Within negative binomial regression models, honor endorsement was significantly positively related to aggressive behavior, while this effect did not differ between students from different migration backgrounds and students without migration background respectively. Thus, the internalization of honor norms emerged as a general risk for aggressive behavior among male adolescents.
Introduction
Irrespective of historical and cultural contexts, criminal involvement is most likely to occur in adolescence compared to any other age (Rocque et al., 2016). In Germany, juvenile delinquency is largely represented by physical injury, a predominantly male phenomenon (Bundeskriminalamt, 2023). According to self-reports, nearly one in four male adolescents in Germany has committed a violent crime at least once in their lifetime (Krieg et al., 2022). During adolescents’ identity formation, normative systems are considered to be most influential in behavioral genesis (Somech & Elizur, 2009). Accordingly, beliefs about the social acceptability of behaviors among a reference group, also apply to violent behavior (Dedios Sanguineti et al., 2023). In studying norms and their variation as part of a particular culture, nationality is often used as a marker of cultural systems.
Given the inferior predictive validity of common risk factors and prognostic instruments for violent delinquency among ethnic minorities (e.g., Olver et al., 2014), it has been discussed that the role of sociocultural norms in shaping everyday life has been neglected so far (Shepherd & Lewis-Fernandez, 2016). Rather, as a general problem of criminal psychology theorizing, authors have raised the issue that theories are formulated primarily for the Euro-American context (Schmidt & Ward, 2021). Hence, the fit of common explanatory models for different cultures urgently needs to be questioned. Furthermore, it should be considered whether risk and protective factor constellations for aggressive behavior apply to the nowadays culturally diverse German population.
In 2020, almost 40% of children and young people under the age of 18 had a so-called migrant background, which is defined as either the child or at least one of their parents being born without German citizenship. The most common countries of origin for these individuals or their parents were Turkey, Russia, and Poland (Statistisches Bundesamt, 2022).
Specifically, for young male violent offenders from a Muslim cultural background in Germany, prognostic instruments did not provide a valid prediction of recidivism, in contrast to native offenders (Dahle & Schmidt, 2014). Therefore, some authors argue that to explain violent delinquency among specific groups of perpetrators, namely people with Turkish and Arabic migration or Muslim cultural backgrounds, traditional norms of honor must be considered (Dahle & Schmidt, 2014; Schmidt et al., 2017, 2019). To date, it remains to be tested whether the internalization of honor norms differs across cultural backgrounds, and whether its impact on violent behavior is independent of national origin.
Cultural Logics of Dignity, Honor, and Face
Leung and Cohen (2011) distinguish three cultural logics, each of which subsumes specific shared beliefs, values, behaviors, and practices around the cultural themes of dignity, honor or face. Cultural logics establish a normative framework that promotes predictability, facilitates social coordination, prescribes strategies for safety as well as conflict resolution, provides group members with a sense of self-worth (Leung & Cohen, 2011), and evolves as adaptations to environmental conditions (Aslani et al., 2013). In the context of this study, the logics of honor and dignity are particularly relevant. In the cultural logic of dignity, individuals are presumed to have an inalienable self-worth that is primarily intrinsically derived (Leung & Cohen, 2011). A person is considered to have a strong sense of dignity if he or she acts in a self-determined manner and in accordance with his or her inner standards. Behavior within dignity cultures is guided by conscience and underpinned by a strong rule of law to provide protection and justice (Leung & Cohen, 2011). In honor cultures, a person’s self-worth has a distinct external quality in addition to an internal one (Leung & Cohen, 2011). It is defined as “the value of a person in his own eyes, but also in the eyes of his society” (Pitt-Rivers, 1968, p. 21). Honor can be acquired and withdrawn, and must be proven by others. To maintain one’s honor, a person is expected to not tolerate cheating and insults, and to repay the wrong done to him or her (Leung & Cohen, 2011). Honor is conceived as a form of social organization when formal legal structures and the protection of individuals by state institutions are perceived as weak (Cohen & Nisbett, 1994). In describing ideal types of dignity and honor logics, the key differences can be summarized as follows: the value of the person is inalienable vs. socially conferred, individual behavior is constrained by guilt for violating internal norms vs. the shame of public condemnation, and the individual is protected by an effective legal system vs. bonds of reciprocity (Leung & Cohen, 2011; Uskul et al., 2019). The idea of external self-evaluation and the emphasis on social image and reputation universally apply to individuals in honor cultures, but the behaviors that honor codes prescribe as appropriate vary by culture and gender (Rodriguez Mosquera, 2011). Female honor norms tend to focus on values of modesty and decency, while male honor codes emphasize strength, toughness, and physical protection (Rodriguez Mosquera, 2016).
All cultural logics are simultaneously accessible, but some may be activated more often than others (Oysermann, 2017; Oyserman & Lee, 2008). If many individuals rely on the same logic, this logic becomes more likely to be seen as normative. Germany, representing a modern Western individualistic and egalitarian society, is expected to be relatively familiar with the dignity motivational system (Leung & Cohen, 2011), whereas the normative system of honor is expected to predominate in Turkey (Uskul et al., 2012), Russia (Helkama et al., 2012; Friedrichs, 2016), and Poland (Krys et al., 2017; Świdrak et al., 2019; Zdybek & Walczak, 2020). Most research on honor has focused on comparisons within the United States or between Western dignity cultures and southern and southeastern European honor cultures (e.g., Spain and Turkey; Uskul et al., 2019). The few studies on Polish honor culture suggest that compared to Norwegians, Poles evaluate themselves more externally (Świdrak et al., 2019), aggression is the preferred response to provocation (Krys et al., 2017), and general gender roles maintain men as active users of violence in response to provocation (Zdybek & Walczak, 2020). Compared to Finland, Estonia, Italy, and Switzerland, honor was considered a more important cultural value among Russian participants (Helkama et al., 2021). The present study extends previous research by focusing on immigrants from different honor cultures within the German majority society, which is expected to emphasize the dignity motivational system.
Even within normative cultures, individuals vary in the extent to which they act in accordance with the surrounding cultural logic. According to Leung and Cohen (2011), “[...] people are active agents within a cultural system, responding to or against the dominant organizing syndrome of their culture” (p. 523). Moreover, individuals can be socialized into different microcultures that support or oppose the norms of the larger society (Anderson, 1994) and given growing multiculturalism, there are increasing opportunities to interact with groups that adhere to different social norms and thus cultural logics.
Honor Endorsement and Aggressive Behavior
The Significance-Quest Theory (Kruglanski et al., 2014, 2022) defines the motivational need to have a social worth and to matter to others as central. As a result of genetics, socialization, and culture, the need for significance differs individually. This need is typically satisfied by measuring up to a given value treasured by the community or culture, the norms of which one has internalized. In the case of a (threatened) loss of significance, such as through evidence of disrespect or incompetence, or the opportunity of significance gain, a quest for significance can be activated. Thus, in order to restore or enhance the sense of belonging, values that are shared with significant others will be provoked. Thereby, cultural narratives have the dual effect of shaping values when significance is quested, and of linking values to actions taken to achieve them (Kruglanski et al., 2022). With higher internalized honor norms, individuals are more sensitive to rejection and insult, tend to feel thereby more attacked, and are more likely to experience significance-loss (Kruglanski, 2014). Since men from honor cultures have been found to endorse violent behavior for self-protection, defense of honor, or socialization of children (Cohen & Nisbet, 1994), it is expected that narratives in honor cultures will code aggressive behavior as significance-lending. The Significance-Quest-Theory is widely supported empirically, indicating greater tendencies to respond aggressively to perceived insults and threats, greater likelihood to carry weapons for protection, more positive perceptions of war, and greater support for aggressive security policies when honor is endorsed (Cohen et al., 1996; Felson & Pare, 2010; Krys et al., 2017; Saucier et al., 2018).
The Cultural Agency-Model of Criminal Behavior (CAMCB; Schmidt et al., 2021; Schmidt & Ward, 2021) also considers cultural influences in the development of violent behavior. The impact of culture is conceptualized through cultural traits that develop as adaptive responses to the demands of the social, institutional, physical, and historical environment (Kim & Lawrie, 2019) and are acquired through social learning processes in order to achieve desired goals and avoid undesired ones (Mesquita et al., 2016). Thus, in this model, cultural influences on aggressive behavior are conceived as highly functional adaptations to environmental features without reference to an individual’s national background.
The Present Study
Summing up, violent crime is a common phenomenon among Germany’s young male population, which is becoming increasingly culturally diverse. It is assumed that adolescents differ in their inalienable vs. socially conferred self-esteem, which is represented in the cultural logics of dignity and honor. Some authors argue that for a valid risk assessment of violent delinquency in certain groups, traditional norms of honor must also be considered, suggesting its conceptualization as a unique risk factor. Honor norms, once adopted, are likely to be carried with migration (Oyserman, 2017), and strong manifestations of honor norms may persist in new societies regardless of their functionality (Vandello & Cohen, 2003). With reference to the normative classification of Germany, Turkey, Russia, and Poland, we expect to find the following group-level differences in the relative familiarity with the honor motivational system among the young German population:
H1: Students with a Turkish, Russian, or Polish immigrant background are more likely to agree with honor norms than students without a migration background.
According to the models presented above, it can be assumed that the means of fulfilling central needs vary according to cultural socialization (Kruglanski et al., 2022). In the context of high familiarity with an honor motivational system, individual violent behavior may have been learned as socially normative for self-esteem maintenance (Enzmann et al., 2003). Therefore, it is more likely to be performed through the significance attached to the behavior.
H2: Honor endorsement and aggressive behavior are positively correlated.
The learning context of honor norms is thereby assumed to transcend ethnic and national boundaries (Leung & Cohen, 2011). Even in Germany as a dignity culture, microcultures characterized by social disadvantage and marginalization show framework conditions for an organization through honor logic (Enzmann et al., 2003). Thus, we assume that the internalization of honor norms provides an explanation for aggressive behavior regardless of the (national) context in which these norms are acquired. This argumentation leads to the following hypothesis 3:
H3: The effect of internalization of honor norms on the performance of aggressive behavior is not moderated by migration background.
Because migration background and socioeconomic status (SES; Statistisches Bundesamt, 2021) are confounded, and SES is associated with the occurrence of aggressive behavior (Derzon, 2010), SES will be controlled for in the analyses.
Method
Sample
Data stem from the larger representative Communities That Care (CTC) survey, conducted from November 2021 to January 2022 in Lower Saxony, Germany (Soellner et al., 2023). The target population was students in grades six to eleven of all public and private regular schools and the first year of vocational schools. Stratified systematic sampling was used with grade and school type as stratification variables. After adjusting for individuals who did not report their or their parents’ place of birth, data from 774 male students aged 11–18 years (M = 15.37; SD = 1.78) were used for the analysis. 65.8% of the students had no migration background, 3.7% had a Turkish, 7.1% a Russian, 2.3% a Polish background, and 21.1% a migration background that could not be further specified. The data were weighted according to grade level and school type.
Measures
2.2.1Migration Background
Country of birth of the student and his parents was used to operationalize migration background. If Turkey was indicated at least once as the country of birth while Germany was selected for the remaining variables, the students were categorized as being of Turkish origin. The same procedure was used to classify students as having a Russian or Polish migration background. All other information led to the categorization of having an “other migrant background”.
Honor Endorsement
To measure honor the translated and adapted German version (Kollek & Soellner, 2023) of the honor scale from the Social Norms Survey (Frey et al., 2021) was used. The adapted scale consists of five items measuring negative reciprocity, retaliation, and permeable self-family boundaries as aspects of honor (e.g., “You should always punish those that betray you”). Students were asked to agree on a scale from 0 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). The items were aggregated to a mean value. The omega coefficient of the scale is reported in the results section.
Socioeconomic Status
A German version (Lampert et al., 2018) of the MacArthur Scale for adolescents (Goodman et al., 2001) was used to assess socioeconomic status. Students were asked to rank their family on a yardstick meant to represent German society, ranging from 0 (people with the least money, the lowest education, and the worst jobs or no job) to 10 (people with the most money, the highest education, and the best jobs).
Aggressive Behavior
To assess students’ aggressive behavior, items of the German translated and adapted version (Soellner et al., 2018) of the U.S. CTC survey (Arthur et al., 2002) were used. Aggressive behavior was assessed with four items. Students were asked if they had engaged in the following behaviors in the past 12 months (0 = no, 1 = yes): intentionally breaking something that didn’t belong to them, attacking someone, threatening someone, and sometimes carrying a weapon (such as a knife). The four items were aggregated into a sum score.
Implementation of the Survey
Classes were invited to participate through the school administration. Parents had to give prior consent for minors to participate in the survey. The survey was designed as an online classroom survey with a duration of one class period. Students were informed that their participation was voluntary, their answers would be treated anonymously, they could choose not to answer questions, and that data protection would be assured. The study was approved by the Ethical Committee of the University of Hildesheim.
Data Analysis
The statistical software R (v4.2.2; R Core Team, 2022) and the following packages were used: (a) psych (v2.2.9; Revelle, 2022) for the calculation of the descriptive statistics, (b) semTools (v0.5.6; Jorgensen et al., 2022) for the calculation of the omega coefficient, (c) AER (v1.2.10; Kleiber & Zeileis, 2008) to test the count outcome data for overdispersion, and (d) performance (v0.10.2; Lüdecke et al., 2021) to test the count outcome data for zero-inflation. The multiple regression model and the negative binomial generalized linear models (NB GLM) were calculated in MPlus (v8.9; Muthén & Muthén, 1998-2017).
Student data were nested within classes (number of clusters = 122, mean cluster size = 6.34). Since 7.9% of the variance in honor endorsement (ICC = 0.079) could be attributed to between-class variance, a single-level analysis with standard errors adjusted for non-independence of observations was chosen for H1 to account for the nested structure. Less than 5% of the variance in aggressive behavior can be attributed to between-class differences, and thus the nested data structure can be neglected in the analyses of H2 and H3 (Heck et al., 2014).
Multiple regression was used to test H1. Migration background was dummy coded, with the group of students with no immigrant background as the reference category. Maximum likelihood estimation with robust standard errors (MLR) were used and missing data (6.01% of all cases) within this model were addressed using full information maximum likelihood (FIML).
To test H2 and H3, a moderation analysis was conducted with honor endorsement as the predictor, migration background (dummy-coded) as the moderator, and aggressive behavior as the criterion, while controlling for SES. The moderation model was calculated in two steps. First, the main effects were analyzed before the interaction terms were included in the second step.
As the dependent variable consists of non-negative integer values representing the variety of aggressive behavior carried out in the past twelve months, the data are concentrated on a few small discrete values, here 0 to 4, and form a positively skewed heteroskedastic distribution. Thus, a count model was applied (Cameron & Trivedi, 2005).
Within the framework of a generalized linear model (GLM; McCullagh & Nelder, 1989) regression models for a variety of outcome variable types including count data can be handled. Since count data can follow different distributions, a multi-stage model selection procedure was first performed based on conducting score tests (for a description, see Walters, 2007). The data were overdispersed compared to the distribution assumptions of the Poisson GLM, with no excess of zeros expected by the negative binomial model. Hence, a NB GLM was selected (Walters, 2007). The NB regression model was fitted using MLR via Monte Carlo numerical integration. Missing data (20.28% of all cases) were handled with FIML.
Results
3.1Preliminary Analyses
Descriptive statistics and internal consistency coefficient of the metric model variables are shown in Table 1. The honor scale items had a satisfactory level of reliability in the present study (ω= 0.78). Skewness and kurtosis values indicated that SES and honor endorsement were approximately normally distributed, but the distribution of aggressive behavior was extremely skewed, with only 25.8% reporting having committed one or more violent acts in the past 12 months. Students reported a mean socioeconomic status above the midpoint of the scale at 6.37. On average, students tended to be somewhat in disagreement to undecided with the aggregated honor scale (M = 1.5, SD = 0.87) and reported 0.43 (SD = 0.88) aggressive assaults in the period under review. Table 2 shows the sample sizes, means, and standard deviations for the model variables by migration background subgroups.
Table 1
SES | Honor | Aggressive | |
Endorsement | Behavior | ||
Mean | 6.37 | 1.5 | 0.43 |
SD | 1.57 | 0.87 | 0.88 |
Skewness | –0.28 | 0.17 | 2.35 |
Kurtosis | 0.88 | –0.25 | 5.14 |
Omega | – | 0.78 | – |
Table 2
Variable | Without MB | Turkish MB | Russian MB | Polish MB | Other MB | |||||
(n = 509) | (n = 29) | (n = 55) | (n = 18) | (n = 163) | ||||||
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | |
SES | 6.54 | 1.40 | 6.24 | 1.73 | 6.00 | 1.83 | 6.58 | 1.16 | 5.91 | 1.88 |
Honor Endorsement | 1.46 | 0.84 | 2.02 | 0.97 | 1.82 | 0.90 | 1.38 | 0.94 | 1.61 | 0.88 |
Aggressive Behavior | 0.39 | 0.82 | 0.74 | 1.26 | 0.57 | 1.07 | 0.50 | 0.86 | 0.46 | 0.90 |
Note. MB = Migration Background.
Main Analyses
Students with a Turkish and a Russian immigrant background approved of honor norms by 0.51 units (SE(b) = 0.20, p = 0.012) and by 0.35 units (SE(b) = 0.12, p = 0.003) more, respectively, than those without a migrant background (see Table 3). No differences in the internalization of honor norms were found between persons with a Polish migration background (b = –0.03, SE(b) = 0.20, p = 0.887) and persons with an unspecified migration background (b = 0.23, SE(b) = 0.12, p = 0.064) compared to those without a migration background.
Table 3
Variable | b | 95% CI for b | SEa | β | |
LL | UL | ||||
Turkish MBb | 0.51* | 0.11 | 0.91 | 0.20 | 0.11 |
Russian MBb | 0.35*** | 0.12 | 0.58 | 0.12 | 0.11 |
Polish MBb | –0.03 | –0.42 | 0.36 | 0.20 | –0.01 |
Other MBb | 0.23 | –0.01 | 0.47 | 0.12 | 0.11 |
Note. CI = confidence interval; LL = lower limit; UL = upper limit. aComplex survey standard errors were computed using the Hubert-White sandwich estimator. bReference category is students without a migrant background. *p < 0.05. ***p < 0.001.
Table 4 displays the results of the negative binominal regression for the main effects of honor endorsement and migration background (dummy-coded) on aggressive behavior, while controlling for SES. Adolescents’ approval of honor norms was significantly positively related with the performance of aggressive behavior, b = 0.70, SE(b) = 0.09, p < 0.001. Holding all other variables constant, a one-unit increase in agreement with honor norms doubles the likelihood that a person will engage in aggressive behavior, exp(b) = 2.02. Given equal SES and level of endorsement of honor norms, the frequency of aggressive behavior did not differ between migrant groups and students without migration background respectively (all p > 0.140).
Table 4
Variable | b | 95% CI for b | SE | exp(b) | |
LL | UL | ||||
SES (C1) | 0.05 | –0.06 | 0.16 | 0.05 | 1.05 |
Honor Endorsement (X) | 0.70*** | 0.52 | 0.88 | 0.09 | 2.02 |
Turkish MBa (W1) | 0.61 | –0.2 | 1.41 | 0.41 | 1.83 |
Russian MBa (W2) | 0.01 | –0.50 | 0.52 | 0.26 | 1.01 |
Polish MBa (W3) | 0.44 | –0.52 | 1.41 | 0.49 | 1.56 |
Other MBa (W4) | 0.04 | –0.35 | 0.43 | 0.20 | 1.04 |
Note. CI = confidence interval; LL = lower limit; UL = upper limit; exp(b) = exponentiated regression coefficient. aReference category is students without a migrant background. ***p < 0.001.
In a second step, the interaction terms were included in the model (see Table 5). As hypothesized, none of the interaction terms were significant (all p > 0.111). The effect of honor endorsement on aggressive behavior thus not differs between each of the migrant groups and students without migration background.
Table 5
Variable | b | 95% CI for b | SE | exp(b) | |
LL | UL | ||||
SES (C1) | 0.04 | –0.06 | 0.15 | 0.05 | 1.05 |
Honor Endorsement (X) | 0.75*** | 0.50 | 0.97 | 0.13 | 2.12 |
Turkish MBa (W1) | 1.73* | 0.01 | 3.45 | 0.88 | 5.64 |
Russian MBa (W2) | –0.38 | –1.95 | 1.19 | 0.80 | 0.68 |
Polish MBa (W3) | 0.95 | –0.72 | 2.63 | 0.86 | 2.59 |
Other MBa (W4) | 0.28 | –0.60 | 1.16 | 0.45 | 1.32 |
X*W1 | –0.58 | –1.29 | 0.13 | 0.36 | 0.56 |
X*W2 | 0.17 | –0.48 | 0.82 | 0.33 | 1.18 |
X*W3 | –0.32 | –1.03 | 0.38 | 0.36 | 0.72 |
X*W4 | –0.13 | –0.55 | 0.30 | 0.22 | 0.88 |
Note. CI = confidence interval; LL = lower limit; UL = upper limit; exp(b) = exponentiated regression coefficient. aReference category is students without a migrant background. *p < 0.05. ***p < 0.001.
Discussion
The present study investigated differences in the endorsement of honor norms among male adolescents in Germany either with or without a migration background. Further, the relation of approving of these norms and behaving aggressively was examined. It was also questioned whether this relation applies to adolescents independent of their migration backgrounds. Students with Russian and Turkish migrant backgrounds where found to internalize norms of honor on average higher than students without migration background, whereas adolescents of Polish origin did not differ from students without migrant backgrounds. Approving of honor norms turned out to be positively related to behaving aggressively over all groups. It is worth noting that the effect of honor endorsement on aggressive behavior did not differ between students from different migration backgrounds and students without a migration background.
In line with the assumption that Turkey and Russia are closer to the cultural logic of honor (Friedrichs et al., 2016; Uskul et al., 2019), the present findings indicate a higher adherence to honor norms among young Turkish and Russian migrants in Germany. So, honor related values seem to endure migration at least for some time. Since norms are highly dependent on social expectations, they will persist simply by assuming that they are still valid and desirable in the social environment (Vandello et al., 2008), and independent of the norm orientation of the actual residential country. Furthermore, in the sense of segmented assimilation theory (Portes & Zhou, 1993), migrants might assimilate to the specific segment of German society to which they migrate. Since norms vary across social segments, in the context of higher levels of poverty and social deprivation among migrants in Germany (Statistisches Bundesamt, 2021), norms of honor might still be evaluated as functional and thus, normative assimilation seems to appear less necessary. Contrary to expectations, students with a Polish migrant background agreed with honor norms at the same level as students without a migration background. This might reflect the above-average integration of the Polish migrant group into the labor market and society (Loew, 2017), but could also be an artefact due to a lack of statistical power (n = 18).
The internalization of honor norms was significantly associated with self-reported aggressive behavior in male youth, independent of the migration background. Thus, the results suggest that a socially conferred self-worth and strong reciprocity ties are similarly associated with aggressive behavior, regardless of the (national) context in which they were acquired. Hence, the endorsement of honor is one conditional, psychological factor for explaining aggressive behavior. It is therefore important to shed light on the conditions under which honor norms develop, especially in Germany, which is moreover regarded as a normative culture of dignity. One developmental condition is the reliability of and trust in social institutions in the local context (Nowak et al., 2016). The subjectively perceived effectiveness of or trust in public institutions is in turn influenced by socialization processes and individual life situations and might therefore vary widely even within one national culture. In a German sample, trust in institutions was found to be strongly related to educational status, with the less educated being less likely to report trust in institutions (Karnick et al., 2021). Additionally, it was found that across ethnic groups, such honor norms generally emerge among young people in Germany in the context of social disadvantage and marginalization (Enzmann et al., 2003). Therefore, social deprivation, low educational attainment, and low levels of trust in public institutions, seem to foster norms of honor to emerge.
Finally, it is necessary to consider the specifics of the age group in question. Adolescents’ social worlds are becoming larger, more diverse, and more peer-oriented, and peer affiliation is an important source of status (Agnew, 2003). Forming or participating in youth groups organized around norms of honor may offer incentives such as easily identifiable role models in a hierarchical organization, the provision of protection, and the opportunity to exercise power and control over others, particularly in the context of male adolescents’ search for identity and orientation (Somech & Elizur, 2009). This may be reinforced by an environment of low social status, where resources are scarce and opportunities to provide masculinity in prosocial ways (e.g., as a provider) are limited (Somech & Elizur, 2009). Thus, under conditions of social exclusion and marginalization, honor norms might develop as milieu-specific forms of organization among young males in order to cope with developmental tasks typical of adolescence (Enzmann et al., 2003).
Limitation and Future Directions
The cross-sectional design does not allow for inferences about causality between the variables studied. The current design only allows for the identification of potential risks that need to be further confirmed for validity based on evidence from longitudinal studies. Also, the sole use of self-reports may have overestimated the effects found in terms of shared method variance. The survey is embedded in the context of primary prevention needs planning and thus provides a low-threshold definition of aggressive behavior. It remains to be examined whether the internalization of honor norms is also suitable for predicting recidivism among officially registered juvenile violent offenders and thus extreme groups. Furthermore, in this analytical model, honor norms were considered as the only influencing factor. Thus, it remains to be seen whether the endorsement of honor norms contributes to the explanation of aggressive behavior when tested in a more complex explanatory model that includes broader risk factors.
Because honor norms may be differently related to coping with typical adolescent developmental tasks in specific milieus, further research is needed on the persistence of these norms during the transition to adulthood or, more generally, on possible changes over the course of aging. In particular, since perceived peer attitudes and behaviors form the basis of social norms that influence how adolescents behave (Cotter & Smokowski, 2015), the individual and peer-level internalization of these norms in predicting aggressive behavior should be analyzed. This knowledge is relevant for designing effective preventive interventions, as they may indicate to prioritize peer group settings.
Implications
The findings suggest that studying cultural group differences, usually by equating nation and culture, should be done carefully in order to not overgeneralize across national groups. That is to say, divergent norms in subgroups of supposedly homogeneous societies might exist, e.g., honor as the predominating logic in segments of classified broader dignity cultures. The attribution of certain norms and their association with criminal conduct as valid only for migrant groups should also be handled sensitively, as it can promote stigmatization. This is particularly relevant in the context under consideration, as notions of honor are primarily studied in the context of aggressive behavior and crime, and thus seem to have negative connotations, while the theorized warm side of honor with politeness and great positive reciprocity rarely receives attention (Aslani et al., 2013). In principle, it is necessary to recognize the functionality of this normative system within the social and ecological context in which it emerges.
In terms of preventing honor-related violence, it may be advisable to target program audiences based on their level of internalization of honor norms, rather than pre-selecting individuals from a particular cultural background. Although students from Turkish and Russian backgrounds endorsed higher honor norms on average than students without a migration background, the internalization of these norms was found to be a proxy for violence regardless of cultural origin.
Conclusion
In conclusion, the present study provides a comprehensive examination of honor endorsement and its relation to self-reported aggressive behavior in a culturally diverse sample of male adolescents in Germany. The results show that although agreement with honor norms differs between groups according to migration background, its explanatory contribution and thus its potential predictive power for aggressive behavior is equivalent for all groups. Given the same level of honor endorsement, the likelihood of aggressive behavior is independent of migration background. The endorsement of honor norms, thus, emerge as a generally relevant value-based risk factor for the perpetration of aggressive behavior among male adolescents.
References
1 | Agnew, R. ((2003) ) An integrated theory of the adolescent peak in offending. Youth & Society, 34: (3), 263–299. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsurg.2016.07.008 |
2 | Anderson, E. ((1994) ) The code of the streets Atlantic Monthly, 273: (5), 81–94. |
3 | Arthur, M. W. , Hawkins, J. D. , Pollard, J. A. , Catalano, R. F. , Baglioni, A. J. Jr. ((2002) ) Measuring risk and protective factors for substance use, delinquency, and other adolescent problem behaviors: The Communities That Care Youth Survey. Evaluation Review, 26: (6), 575–601. https://doi.org/10.1177/0193841X0202600601 |
4 | Aslani, S. , Ramirez-Marin, J. , Brett, J. , Yao, J. , Semnani-Azad, Z. (2013). Dignity, face, and honor cultures: Implications for negotiation and conflict management. In M. Olekalns &W. L. Adair (Eds.), Handbook of research on negotiation (pp. 249–282). Edward Elgar Publishing. |
5 | Bundeskriminalamt. (2023). Polizeiliche Kriminalstatistik Bundesrepublik Deutschland 2022. Informationen zu Tatverdächtigen (Aufgliederung nach Delikten, Anzahl Tatverdächtige insgesamt, nach Geschlecht, nach Alter) [Police crime statistics for the Federal Republic of Germany 2022. Information on suspects]. Retrieved May 13, 2023, from https://www.bka.de/DE/AktuelleInformationen/StatistikenLagebilder/PolizeilicheKriminalstatistik/PKS2022/PKSTabellen/BundTV/bundTV.html?nn=211724 |
6 | Cameron, A. C. , Trivedi, P. K. (2005). Microeconometrics: Methods and applications. Cambridge University Press. |
7 | Cohen, D. , Nisbett, R. E. ((1994) ) Self-protection and the culture of honor: Explaining southern violence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20: (5)551–567. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167294205012 |
8 | Cohen, D. , Nisbett, R. E. , Bowdle, B. F. , Schwarz, N. ((1996) ) Insult, aggression, and the southern culture of honor: An “experimental ethnography”. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 20: (5) 945–959 https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.70.5.945 |
9 | Cotter, K. L. , Smokowski, P. R. ((2015) ) Perceived peer delinquency and externalizing behavior among rural youth: The role of descriptive norms and internalizing symptoms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 45: (3) 520–531 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-015-0382-1 |
10 | Dahle, K.-P. , Schmidt, S. ((2014) ) Prognostische Validität des Level of Service Inventory-Revised [Prognostic validity of the Level of Service Inventory-Revised]. Forensis-che Psychiatrie, Psychologie, Kriminologie, 8: (2), 104–115. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11757-014-0256-5 |
11 | Dedios Sanguineti M. C. , Yepes Fiallo V. , Valencia Garzón M. J. , Noy Robayo A. , Pugh, J. D. , Vecino Ortiz A. I. ((2023) ) Social norms and interventions on peer violence: A systematic review of the empirical literature. Adolescent Research Review, 8: (1), 63–73https://doi.org/10.1007/s40894-022-00191-8 |
12 | Derzon, J. H. ((2010) ) The correspondence of family features with problem, aggressive, criminal, and violent behavior: A meta-analysis. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 6: (3), 263–292https://doi.org/10.1007/s11292-010-9098-0 |
13 | Enzmann, D. , Brettfeld, K. , Wetzels, P. ((2003) ) (2003). Männlichkeitsnormen und die Kultur der Ehre: Empirische Püufung eines theoretischen Modells zur Erklärung erhöhter Delinquenzraten jugendlicher Migranten [Norms of masculin- ity and the culture of honor: Empirical testing of a theoretical model to explain increased delinquency rates among adoles- cent immigrants]. In D. Oberwittler & S. Karstedt (Eds.), Kölner Zeitschrift für Soziologie und Sozialpsychologie: Vol. 43. Soziologie der Kriminalität, (pp. 264ߝ287) VS Verl. für Sozialwiss. |
14 | Felson R. B. , Pare P.-P. ((2010) ) Gun cultures or honor cultures? Explaining regional and race differences in weapon carrying. Social Forces, 88: (3), 1357–1378https://doi.org/10.1353/sof.0.0310 |
15 | Frey, K. S. , Onyewuenyi, A. C. , Hymel, S. , Gill, R. , Pearson, C. R. ((2021) ) Honor, face, and dignity norm endorsement among diverse North American adolescents: Development of a social norms survey. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 45: (3), 256–268https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025420949690 |
16 | Friedrichs, J. ((2016) ) An intercultural theory of international relations: How self-underlies politics among nations. International Theory, 8: (1), 63–96https://doi.org/10.1017/S1752971915000202 |
17 | Goodman, E. , Adler, N. E. , Kawachi, I. , Frazier, A. L. , Huang, B. , Colditz, G. A. ((2001) ) Adolescents’ perceptions of social status: Development and evaluation of a new indicator. Pediatrics , 108: (2), E31https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.108.2.e31 |
18 | Heck, R. H. , Thomas, S. L. , Tabata, L. N. (2014). Multilevel and longitudinal modeling with IBM SPSS (2nd ed.). Routledge. |
19 | Helkama, K. , Verkasalo, M. , Myyry, L. , Silfver, M. , Niit, T. , Manganelli, A.-M. , Andreeva, G. M. , Stefanenko, T. G. , Dubovskaya, E. M. , Tikhomandritskaya, O. A. , Stetsenko, A. ((2012) ) Honor as a value in Finland, Estonia, Italy, Russia, and Switzerland. Group Processes&Intergroup Relations16: (3), 279–297 https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430212463452 |
20 | Jorgenens, T. D. , Pornprasertmanit, S. , Schoemann, A. M. , Rosseel, Y. (2022). semTools: Useful tools for structural equation modeling (Version 0.5-6) [Computer software].https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=semTools |
21 | Karnick, N. , Simonson, J. , Tesch-Römer, C. (2021). Ein stellungen gegenüber gesellschaftlichen Institutionen und der Demokratie [Attitudes towards social institutions and democracy]. In J. Simonson, C. Kausmann,&C. Tesch-Römer (Eds.), Freiwilliges Engagement in Deutschland: Der Deutsche Freiwilligensurvey 2019 (pp. 252–275). Deutsches Zentrum fuür Altersfragen |
22 | Kim, H. S. , Lawrie, S. I. (2019). Culture and motivation. In D. Cohen&S. Kitayama (Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology (pp. 268–291). The Guilford Press. |
23 | Kleiber, C. , Zeileis, A. (2008). Applied econometrics with R. Springer-Verlag. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=AER |
24 | Kollek, M. , Soellner, R. (2023). Measuring honor endorsement in youth: Adaptation of a scale for the German language area [Manuscript in preparation]. Department of Psychology, University of Hildesheim. |
25 | Krieg, Y. , Rook, L. , Beckmann, L. , Kliem, S. (2022). Adolescents in Lower Saxony: Results of the Lower Saxony Survey 2019. Research Report (Vol. 165). KFN. |
26 | Kruglanski, A. W. , Gelfand, M. J. , Bélanger, J. J. , Sheveland, A. , Hetiarachchi, M. , Gunaratna, R. ((2014) ) The psychology of radicalization and deradicalization: How significance quest impacts violent extremism. Political Psychology , 35: , 69–93https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12163 |
27 | Kruglanski, A. W. , Molinario, E. , Jasko, K. , Webber, D. , Leander, N. P. , Pierro, A. ((2022) ) Significance-quest theory. Perspectives on Psychological Science , 17: (4), 1050–1071https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916211034825 |
28 | Krys, K. , Xing, C. , Zelenski, J. M. , Capaldi, C. A. , Lin, Z. , Wojciszke, B. ((2017) ) Punches or punchlines? Honor, face, and dignity cultures encourage different reactions to provocation. Humor, 30: (3), 303–322https://doi.org/10.1515/humor-2016-0087 |
29 | Lampert, T. , Hoebel, J. , Kuntz, B. , Müters, S. , Kroll, L. E. ((2018) ) Messung des sozioökonomischen Status und des subjektiven sozialen Status in KiGGS Welle [Measurement of socioeconomic status and subjective social status in KiGGS wave 2.]. Journal of Health Monitoring, 3: (1), 114–133https://doi.org/10.17886/RKI-GBE-2018-016 |
30 | Leung, A. K.-Y. , Cohen, D. ((2011) ) Within- and between-culture variation: Individual differences and the cultural logics of honor, face, and dignity cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100: (3), 507–526https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022151 |
31 | Loew, P. O. (2017). Unsichtbar? Polinnen und Polen in Deutschland – die zweitgröβte Zuwanderergruppe [Invisible? Poles in Germany - the second largest immigrant group]. Bundeszentrale für politische Bildung. Retrieved May 13, 2023, from https://www.bpb.de/themen/migrationintegration/kurzdossiers/256398/unsichtbar-polinnen-undpolen-in-deutschland-die-zweitgroesste-zuwanderergruppe/ |
32 | Lüdecke, D. , Ben-Shachar, M. S. , Patil, I. , Waggoner, P. , Makowski, D. ((2021) ) performance: An R package for assessment, comon and testing of statistical models. Journal of Open Source Software, 6: (60), 3139https://doi.org/10.21105/joss.03139 |
33 | McCullagh, P. , Nelder, J. A. (1989). Generalized linear models (2nd ed.). Chapman & Hall |
34 | Mesquita, B. , Boiger, M. , De Leersnyder J. ((2016) ) The cultural construction of emotions. Current Opinion in Psychology, 8: , 31–36https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.09.015 |
35 | Muthén L. K. , Muthén B. O. , (1998-2017). Mplus user’s guide (8th ed.). Muthén & Muthén. |
36 | Nowak, A. , Gelfand, M. J. , Borkowski, W. , Cohen, D. , Hernandez, I. ((2016) ) The evolutionary basis of honor cultures. Psychological Science, 27: (1), 12–24https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615602860 |
37 | Olver, M. E. , Stockdale, K. C. , Wormith, J. S. ((2014) ) Thirty years of research on the level of service scales: A meta-analytic examination of predictive accuracy and sources of variability. Psychological Assessment, 26: (1), 156–176https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035080 |
38 | Oyserman, D. ((2017) ) Culture three ways: Culture and subcultures within countries. Annual Review of Psychology , 68: , 435–463 https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033617 |
39 | Oyserman, D. , Lee, S. W. S. ((2008) ) Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin , 134: (2), 311–342 https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.311 |
40 | Pitt-Rivers, J. (1968). Honor. In D. Sills (Ed.), International ency-clopedia of the social sciences (pp. 509–510). Macmillan. |
41 | Portes, A. , Zhou, M. ((1993) ) The new second generation: Segmented assimilation and its variants. Annals of the Amer- ican Academy of Political and Social Science , 530: (1), 74–96 https://doi.org/10.1177/0002716293530001006 |
42 | R Core Team.. (2022). R:Alanguage and environment for statistical computing (Version 4.2.2) [Computer software]. R Foundation for Statistical Computing.https://www.R-project.org/ |
43 | Revelle, W. (2022). psych: Procedures for psychological, psycho-metric, and personality research (Version 2.2.9) [Computer software].https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych |
44 | Rocque, M. , Posick, C. , Hoyle, J. (2016). Age and crime. InW. G. Jennings (Ed.), The encyclopedia of crime and punishment (pp. 1–8). Wiley Blackwell. |
45 | Rodriguez Mosquera P. M. ((2011) ) Masculine and feminine honor codes. Revista de Psicología Social, 26: (1), 63–72 https://doi.org/10.1174/021347411794078499 |
46 | Rodriguez Mosquera P. M. ((2016) ) On the importance of family, morality, masculine, and feminine honor for theory and research. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 10: (8), 431–442 https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12262 |
47 | Saucier, D. A. , Webster, R. J. , McManus, J. L. , Sonnentag, T. L. , O’Dea, C. J. , Strain, M. L. ((2018) ) Individual differences in masculine honor beliefs predict attitudes toward aggressive security measures, war, and peace. Journal of Peace Psychology, 24: (1), 112–116 https://doi.org/10.1037/pac0000303 |
48 | Schmidt, S. , Bliesener, T. , van der Meer, E. ((2019) ) Risk and protective factors of delinquency that are sensitive to migration and culture. Psychology, Crime & Law , 25: (8), 847–873 https://doi.org/10.1080/1068316X.2019.1597088 |
49 | Schmidt, S. , Heffernan, R. , Ward, T. ((2021) ) The cultural-agency model of criminal behavior. Aggression & Violent Behavior, 58: .https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2021.101554 |
50 | Schmidt, S. , van der Meer E. , Tydecks, S. , Bliesener, T. ((2017) ) Wie lässt sich Delinquenz bei Personen mit türkischem oder arabischem Migrationshintergrund erklären? [How can delinquency be explained among persons with a Turkish or Arab immigrant background?]. Forensische Psychiatrie, Psychologie, Kriminologie, 11: (4), 304–321 https://doi.org/10.1007/s11757-017-0443-2 |
51 | Schmidt, S. , Ward, T. ((2021) ) Delinquenz kultursensibel erklären –ein theoretisches Rahmenmodell [Explaining delinquency in a culturally sensitive way - a theoretical framework model]. Forensische Psychiatrie, Psychologie, Kriminologie , 15: (2), 143–158 https://doi.org/10.1007/s11757-020-00638-5 |
52 | Shepherd, S. M. , Lewis-Fernandez, R. ((2016) ) Forensic risk assessment and cultural diversity: Contemporary challenges and future directions. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 22: (4), 427–438 https://doi.org/10.1037/law0000102 |
53 | Soellner, R. , Reder, M. , Frisch, J. U. (2018). Communities That Care: Schülerbefragung in Niedersachsen 2017 [Communities That Care: Student survey in Lower Saxony 2017]. Universitätsverlag Hildesheim.https://doi.org/10.18442/802 |
54 | Soellner, R. , Schlüter, H. , Kollek, M. (2023). Communities That Care: Jugendbefragung in Niedersachsen 2021/2022 [Communities That Care: Youth survey in Lower Saxony 2021/2022] (2nd ed.). Universitätsverlag Hildesheim.https://doi.org/10.18442/234 |
55 | Somech, L. Y. , Elizur, Y. ((2009) ) Adherence to honor code mediates the prediction of adolescent boys’ conduct problems by callousness and socioeconomic status. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 38: (5), 606–618 https://doi.org/10.1080/15374410903103593 |
56 | Statistisches Bundesamt..(2021). Datenreport 2021: Ein Sozial bericht für die Bundesrepublik Deutschland [Data Report 2021: A Social Report for the Federal Republic of Germany]. Bundeszentrale für politische Bildung.https://www.destatis.de/DE/Service/Statistik-Campus/Datenreport/Downloads/datenreport-2021.pdf? blob=publicationFile |
57 | Statistisches Bundesamt.. (2022). Bevölkerung und Erwerbstätigkeit: Bevölkerung mit Migrationshintergrund – Ergebnisse des Mikrozensus 2020 [Population and employment: Population with a migration background – Results of the 2020 microcensus]. Fachserie 1 Reihe 2.2. https://www.destatis.de/DE/Themen/Gesellschaft-Umwelt/Bevoelkerung/Migration-Integration/Publikationen/Downloads-Migration/migrationshintergrund-2010220207004.pdf?blob=publicationFile |
58 | Świdrak, J. , Krys, K. , Kwiatkowska, A. , Kosakowska-Berezecka, N. , Gjerstad, B. ((2019) ) Polish honor and Norwegian dignity: Life satisfaction, acculturation, and self-worth. Acta de Investigación Psicológica , 9: (3), 5–13 https://doi.org/10.22201/fpsi.20074719e.2019.3.316 |
59 | Uskul, A. K. , Cross, S. E. , Gunsoy, C. , Gul, P. (2019). Cultures of honor. InD. Cohen&S. Kitayama (Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology (2nd ed., pp. 793–821). The Guilford Press. |
60 | Uskul, A. K. , Cross, S. E. , Sunbay, Z. , Gercek-Swing, B. , Ataca, B. ((2012) ) Honor bound: The cultural construction of honor in Turkey and the Northern United States. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology , 43: (7), 1131–1151 https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022111422258 |
61 | Vandello, J. A. , Cohen, D. (2003).Whenbelieving is seeing: Sus-taining norms of violence in cultures of honor. InM. Schaller& C. S. Crandall (Eds.), The psychological foundations of culture (pp. 290–313). Lawrence Erlbaum. |
62 | Vandello, J. A. , Cohen, D. , Ransom, S. ((2008) ) U.S. Southern and Northern differences in perceptions of norms about aggression: Mechanisms for the perpetuation of a culture of honor. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology , 39: (2), 162–177 https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022107313862 |
63 | Walters, G. D. ((2007) ) Using Poisson class regression to analyze count data in correctional and forensic Psychology: A relatively old solution to a relatively new problem. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 34: (12), 1659–1674 https://doi.org/10.1177/0093854807307030 |
64 | Zdybek, P. M. , Walczak, R. B. ((2020) ) Does the culture of honor do well in Poland. Family Forum, 9: , 113–130 https://doi.org/10.25167/FF/1289 |
Bio Sketches
Marie Kollek, Ph.D. candidate at the Division of Evaluation and Research Methods, Department of Psychology at the University of Hildesheim, conducts cross-cultural research on risk and protective factors for adolescent externalizing problem behavior and the concept of mental disorder.
Dr. Renate Soellner, Professor for Methodology at the University of Hildesheim and Head of the Division “Evaluation and Research Methods”, is engaged in the research on evaluation of health prevention programs and juvenile substance use.